Paper 2 - Research Methods

Paper 2 research methods, as/a level revision notes (aqa), research methods resources, quantitative and qualitative data: the distinction between qualitative and quantitative data collection techniques.

Paper 2 - Psychology in Context | Research Methods | 30 Minutes

Variables In Psychological Research

Experimental methods in psychology, aims and hypotheses, directional and non-directional​, sampling methods, experimental designs, ethics; including the role of the british psychological society’s code of ethics, correlational analysis: positive, negative and zero correlations, naturalistic observation, questionnaire construction; including the use of open and closed questions, design of interviews, case studies, pilot study, reliability, features of science, objectivity and the empirical method, psychological report writing, primary and secondary data, including meta-analysis, descriptive statistics: measures of central tendency (mean, median and mode), presentation and display of quantitative data: graphs, tables, scatter grams and bar charts, measures of dispersion (range and standard deviation), calculating percentages, quantitative data analysis - normal and skewed distributions, sign test – inferential statistics, statistical (inferential) testing​, qualitative data analysis, the role of peer review in the scientific process​.

We're not around right now. But you can send us an email and we'll get back to you, asap.

PMT Education is looking for a full-time Customer Support Specialist

PMT

AQA A-level Psychology Revision

A-level paper 1, a-level paper 2, a-level paper 3.

  • Revision Courses
  • Past Papers
  • Solution Banks
  • University Admissions
  • Numerical Reasoning
  • Legal Notices

AQA A-Level Psychology Past Papers

AQA A-Level ​Psychology (7182) and AS-Level Psychology (7181) past exam papers. You can download the papers and marking schemes by clicking on the links below. Scroll down to find papers from previous years.

June 2023 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers

A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme A-Level Psychology Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology (7182/3) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme  

June 2023 - AQA AS-Level Psychology (7181) Past Papers

AS Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7181/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

AS Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7181/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2022 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers

November 2021 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers (Labelled as June 2021)

November 2021 A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme November 2021 A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme November 2021 A-Level Psychology Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology (7182/3) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

November 2020 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers (Labelled as June 2020)

November 2020 A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme November 2020 A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme November 2020 A-Level Psychology Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology (7182/3) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme  

November 2020 - AQA AS-Level Psychology (7181) Past Papers (Labelled as June 2020)

November 2020 AS Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7181/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

November 2020 AS Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7181/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2019 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers

June 2019 A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme June 2019 A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme June 2019 A-Level Psychology Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology (7182/3) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2019 - AQA AS-Level Psychology (7181) Past Papers

June 2019 AS Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7181/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2019 AS Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7181/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2018 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers

June 2018 A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme June 2018 A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme June 2018 A-Level Psychology Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology (7182/3) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2018 - AQA AS-Level Psychology (7181) Past Papers

June 2018 AS Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7181/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2018 AS Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7181/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

A-Level Psychology Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology (7182/3) Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme

June 2017 - AQA AS-Level Psychology (7181) Past Papers

AS Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7181/1)

Download Past Paper    -    Download Marking Scheme  

For more A-Level Psychology past papers from other exam boards  click here .

sign up to revision world banner

A-level Psychology AQA Revision Notes

Saul McLeod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul McLeod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology

  • AS : written exam: 1 hour 30 minutes: 72 marks in total: 50% of AS
  • A-level : written exam: 2 hours: 96 marks in total: 33.3% of A-level
1. Social Influence (24 marks) 2. Memory (24 marks) 3. Attachment (24 marks) 4. Psychopathology A-Level Only (24 marks) multiple choice, short answer and extended writing

Paper 2: Psychology in Context

  • AS : Compulsory content 1–3: written exam: 1 hour 30 minutes: 72 marks in total: 50% of AS
5. Approaches to Psychology (24 marks) 6. Biopsychology (24 marks) 7. Research Methods ( AS : 24 marks & A-level : 48 marks) multiple choice, short answer and extended writing

Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology

  • written exam: 2 hours: 96 marks in total: 33.3% of A-level
8. Issues and Debates (24 marks) All students answer this question
9. Relationships (24 marks) 10. Gender (24 marks) 11. Cognition and Development (24 marks) one from option 1, 9–11
12. Schizophrenia (24 marks) 13. Eating Behavior (24 marks) 14. Stress (24 marks) one from option 2, 12–14
15. Aggression (24 marks) 16. Forensic Psychology (24 marks) 17. Addiction (24 marks) one from option 3, 15–17

Assessment Objectives

There are three assessment objectives assessed in each examination: 
  • Demonstrate knowledge
  • Application of knowledge
  • Analyse, interpret, and evaluate

There may be one, two, or all (only in the extended writing 16-mark question).

AO1: Demonstrate Knowledge

  • Demonstrate knowledge and understanding of scientific ideas, processes, techniques, and procedures.
  • Show knowledge and understanding of psychological theories, terminology, concepts, studies, and methods.

AO2: Application of Knowledge

In application questions, examiners look for “effective application to the scenario, ” meaning that you need to describe the theory and explain the scenario using the theory, making the links between the two very clear.

If there is more than one individual in the scenario, you must mention all the characters to get to the top band.

AO3: Analyse, interpret and evaluate

Analyse, interpret, and evaluate scientific information, ideas, and evidence, including in relation to issues, to:

  • make judgements and reach conclusions
  • develop and refine practical design and procedures.

For example :

  • Whether or not theories are supported or refuted by valid research evidence.
  • General criticisms and/or strengths of theories and studies:
  • Contextualising how the topic in question relates to broader debates and approaches in Psychology
  • Animal Research raises the issue of whether it’s morally and/or scientifically right to use animals. The main criterion is that benefits must outweigh costs. Animal research also raises the issue of extrapolation. Can we generalize from studies on animals to humans, as their anatomy and physiology are different from humans?

Download PDFs Resources

  • Attachment Revision Notes
  • Memory Revision Notes
  • Social Influence Revision Notes
  • Psychopathology Revision Notes

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Psychology A Level

Overview – Research Methods

Research methods are how psychologists and scientists come up with and test their theories. The A level psychology syllabus covers several different types of studies and experiments used in psychology as well as how these studies are conducted and reported:

  • Types of psychological studies (including experiments , observations , self-reporting , and case studies )
  • Scientific processes (including the features of a study , how findings are reported , and the features of science in general )
  • Data handling and analysis (including descriptive statistics and different ways of presenting data ) and inferential testing

Note: Unlike all other sections across the 3 exam papers, research methods is worth 48 marks instead of 24. Not only that, the other sections often include a few research methods questions, so this topic is the most important on the syllabus!

aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

Example question: Design a matched pairs experiment the researchers could conduct to investigate differences in toy preferences between boys and girls. [12 marks]

Types of study

There are several different ways a psychologist can research the mind, including:

  • Experiments
  • Observation
  • Self-reporting

Case studies

Each of these methods has its strengths and weaknesses. Different methods may be better suited to different research studies.

Experimental method

The experimental method looks at how variables affect outcomes. A variable is anything that changes between two situations ( see below for the different types of variables ). For example, Bandura’s Bobo the doll experiment looked at how changing the variable of the role model’s behaviour affected how the child played.

Experimental designs

Experiments can be designed in different ways, such as:

  • Independent groups: Participants are divided into two groups. One group does the experiment with variable 1, the other group does the experiment with variable 2. Results are compared.
  • Repeated measures: Participants are not divided into groups. Instead, all participants do the experiment with variable 1, then afterwards the same participants do the experiment with variable 2. Results are compared.

A matched pairs design is another form of independent groups design. Participants are selected. Then, the researchers recruit another group of participants one-by-one to match the characteristics of each member of the original group. This provides two groups that are relevantly similar and controls for differences between groups that might skew results. The experiment is then conducted as a normal independent groups design.

Types of experiment

Laboratory vs. field experiment.

Experiments are carried out in two different types of settings:

  • E.g. Bandura’s Bobo the doll experiment or Asch’s conformity experiments
  • E.g. Bickman’s study of the effects of uniforms on obedience

Strengths of laboratory experiment over field experiment:

The controlled environment of a laboratory experiment minimises the risk of other variables outside the researchers’ control skewing the results of the trial, making it more clear what (if any) the causal effects of a variable are. Because the environment is tightly controlled, any changes in outcome must be a result of a change in the variable.

Weaknesses of laboratory experiment over field experiment:

However, the controlled nature of a laboratory experiment might reduce its ecological validity . Results obtained in an artificial environment might not translate to real-life. Further, participants may be influenced by demand characteristics : They know they are taking part in a test, and so behave how they think they’re expected to behave rather than how they would naturally behave.

Natural and quasi experiment

Natural experiments are where variables vary naturally. In other words, the researcher can’t or doesn’t manipulate the variables . There are two types of natural experiment:

  • E.g. studying the effect a change in drug laws (variable) has on addiction
  • E.g. studying differences between men (variable) and women (variable)

Observational method

The observational method looks at and examines behaviour. For example, Zimbardo’s prison study observed how participants behaved when given certain social roles.

Observational design

Behavioural categories.

An observational study will use behavioural categories to prioritise which behaviours are recorded and ensure the different observers are consistent in what they are looking for.

For example, a study of the effects of age and sex on stranger anxiety in infants might use the following behavioural categories to organise observational data:

A F 2 IS 2
B F 1 AS 4
C M 3 IS 1
D F 2 IS 2
E M 2 AS 6

Rather than writing complete descriptions of behaviours, the behaviours can be coded into categories. For example, IS = interacted with stranger, and AS = avoided stranger. Researchers can also create numerical ratings to categorise behaviour, like the anxiety rating example above.

Inter-observer reliability : In order for observations to produce reliable findings, it is important that observers all code behaviour in the same way. For example, researchers would have to make it very clear to the observers what the difference between a ‘3’ on the anxiety scale above would be compared to a ‘7’. This inter-observer reliability avoids subjective interpretations of the different observers skewing the findings.

Event and time sampling

Because behaviour is constant and varied, it may not be possible to record every single behaviour during the observation period. So, in addition to categorising behaviour , study designers will also decide when to record a behaviour:

  • Event sampling: Counting how many times the participant behaves in a certain way.
  • Time sampling: Recording participant behaviour at regular time intervals. For example, making notes of the participant’s behaviour after every 1 minute has passed.

Note: Don’t get event and time sampling confused with participant sampling , which is how researchers select participants to study from a population.

Types of observation

Naturalistic vs. controlled.

Observations can be made in either a naturalistic or a controlled setting:

  • E.g. setting up cameras in an office or school to observe how people interact in those environments
  • E.g. Ainsworth’s strange situation or Zimbardo’s prison study

Covert vs. overt

Observations can be either covert or overt :

  • E.g. setting up hidden cameras in an office
  • E.g. Zimbardo’s prison study

Participant vs. non-participant

In observational studies, the researcher/observer may or may not participate in the situation being observed:

  • E.g. in Zimbardo’s prison study , Zimbardo played the role of prison superintendent himself
  • E.g. in Bandura’s Bobo the doll experiment and Ainsworth’s strange situation , the observers did not interact with the children being observed

Self-report method

Self-report methods get participants to provide information about themselves. Information can be obtained via questionnaires or interviews .

Types of self-report

Questionnaires.

A questionnaire is a standardised list of questions that all participants in a study answer. For example, Hazan and Shaver used questionnaires to collate self-reported data from participants in order to identify correlations between attachment as infants and romantic attachment as adults.

Questions in a questionnaire can be either open or closed :

  • >8 hours
  • E.g. “How did you feel when you thought you were administering a lethal shock?” or “What do you look for in a romantic partner and why?”

Strengths of questionnaires:

  • Quantifiable: Closed questions provide quantifiable data in a consistent format, which enables to statistically analyse information in an objective way.
  • Replicability: Because questionnaires are standardised (i.e. pre-set, all participants answer the same questions), studies involving them can be easily replicated . This means the results can be confirmed by other researchers, strengthening certainty in the findings.

Weaknesses of questionnaires:

  • Biased samples: Questionnaires handed out to people at random will select for participants who actually have the time and are willing to complete the questionnaire. As such, the responses may be biased towards those of people who e.g. have a lot of spare time.
  • Dishonest answers: Participants may lie in their responses – particularly if the true answer is something they are embarrassed or ashamed of (e.g. on controversial topics or taboo topics like sex)
  • Misunderstanding/differences in interpretation: Different participants may interpret the same question differently. For example, the “are you religious?” example above could be interpreted by one person to mean they go to church every Sunday and pray daily, whereas another person may interpret religious to mean a vague belief in the supernatural.
  • Less detail: Interviews may be better suited for detailed information – especially on sensitive topics – than questionnaires. For example, participants are unlikely to write detailed descriptions of private experiences in a questionnaire handed to them on the street.

In an interview , participants are asked questions in person. For example, Bowlby interviewed 44 children when studying the effects of maternal deprivation.

Interviews can be either structured or unstructured :

  • Structured interview: Questions are standardised and pre-set. The interviewer asks all participants the same questions in the same order.
  • Unstructured interview: The interviewer discusses a topic with the participant in a less structured and more spontaneous way, pursuing avenues of discussion as they come up.

Interviews can also be a cross between the two – these are called semi-structured interviews .

Strengths of interviews:

  • More detail: Interviews – particularly unstructured interviews conducted by a skilled interviewer – enable researchers to delve deeper into topics of interest, for example by asking follow-up questions. Further, the personal touch of an interviewer may make participants more open to discussing personal or sensitive issues.
  • Replicability: Structured interviews are easily replicated because participants are all asked the same pre-set list of questions. This replicability means the results can be confirmed by other researchers, strengthening certainty in the findings.

Weaknesses of interviews:

  • Lack of quantifiable data: Although unstructured interviews enable researchers to delve deeper into interesting topics, this lack of structure may produce difficulties in comparing data between participants. For example, one interview may go down one avenue of discussion and another interview down a different avenue. This qualitative data may make objective or statistical analysis difficult.
  • Interviewer effects : The interviewer’s appearance or character may bias the participant’s answers. For example, a female participant may be less comfortable answering questions on sex asked by a male interviewer and and thus give different answers than if she were asked by a female interviewer.

Note: This topic is A level only, you don’t need to learn about case studies if you are taking the AS exam only.

Case studies are detailed investigations into an individual, a group of people, or an event. For example, the biopsychology page describes a case study of a young boy who had the left hemisphere of his brain removed and the effects this had on his language skills.

In a case study, researchers use many of the methods described above – observation , questionnaires , interviews – to gather data on a subject. However, because case studies are studies of a single subject, the data they provide is primarily qualitative rather than quantitative . This data is then used to build a case history of the subject. Researchers then interpret this case history to draw their conclusions.

Types of case study

Typical vs. unusual cases.

Most case studies focus on unusual individuals, groups, and events.

Longitudinal

Many case studies are longitudinal . This means they take place over an extended time period, with researchers checking in with the subject at various intervals. For example, the case study of the boy who had his left hemisphere removed collected data on the boy’s language skills at ages 2.5, 4, and 14 to see how he progressed.

Strengths of case studies:

  • Provides detailed qualitative data: Rather than focusing on one or two aspects of behaviour at a single point in time (e.g. in an experiment ), case studies produce detailed qualitative data.
  • Allows for investigation into issues that may be impractical or unethical to study otherwise. For example, it would be unethical to remove half a toddler’s brain just to experiment , but if such a procedure is medically necessary then researchers can use this opportunity to learn more about the brain.

Weaknesses of case studies:

  • Lack of scientific rigour: Because case studies are often single examples that cannot be replicated , the results may not be valid when applied to the general population.
  • Researcher bias: The small sample size of case studies also means researchers need to apply their own subjective interpretation when drawing conclusions from them. As such, these conclusions may be skewed by the researcher’s own bias and not be valid when applied more generally. This criticism is often directed at Freud’s psychoanalytic theory because it draws heavily on isolated case studies of individuals.

Scientific processes

This section looks at how science works more generally – in particular how scientific studies are organised and reported . It also covers ways of evaluating a scientific study.

Study features and design

Studies will usually have an aim . The aim of a study is a description of what the researchers are investigating and why . For example, “to investigate the effect of SSRIs on symptoms of depression” or “to understand the effect uniforms have on obedience to authority”.

Studies seek to test a hypothesis . The experimental/alternate hypothesis of a study is a testable prediction of what the researchers expect to happen.

  • E.g. “That SSRIs will reduce symptoms of depression” or “subjects are more likely to comply when orders are issued by someone wearing a uniform”.
  • E.g. “That SSRIs have no effect on symptoms on depression” or “subject conformity will be the same when orders are issued by someone wearing a uniform as when orders are issued by someone bot wearing a uniform”

Either the experimental/alternate hypothesis or the null hypothesis will be supported by the results of the experiment.

It’s often not possible or practical to conduct research on everyone your study is supposed to apply to. So, researchers use sampling to select participants for their study.

  • E.g. all humans, all women, all men, all children, etc.
  • E.g. 10,000 humans, 200 women from the USA, children at a certain school

For example, the target population (i.e. who the results apply to) of Asch’s conformity experiments is all humans – but Asch didn’t conduct the experiment on that many people! Instead, Asch recruited 123 males and generalised the findings from this sample to the rest of the population.

Researchers choose from different sampling techniques – each has strengths and weaknesses.

Sampling techniques

Random sampling.

The random sampling method involves selecting participants from a target population at random – such as by drawing names from a hat or using a computer program to select them. This method means each member of the population has an equal chance of being selected and thus is not subject to any bias.

Strengths of random sampling:

  • Unbiased: Selecting participants by random chance reduces the likelihood that researcher bias will skew the results of the study.
  • Representative: If participants are selected at random – particularly if the sample size is large – it is likely that the sample will be representative of the population as a whole. For example, if the ratio of men:women in a population is 50:50 and participants are selected at random, it is likely that the sample will also have a ratio of men to women that is 50:50.

Weaknesses of random sampling:

  • Impractical: It’s often impractical/impossible to include all members of a target population for selection. For example, it wouldn’t be feasible for a study on women to include the name of every woman on the planet for selection. But even if this was done, the randomly selected women may not agree to take part in the study anyway.

Systematic sampling

The systematic sampling method involves selecting participants from a target population by selecting them at pre-set intervals. For example, selecting every 50th person from a list, or every 7th, or whatever the interval is.

Strengths of systematic sampling:

  • Unbiased and representative: Like random sampling , selecting participants according to a numerical interval provides an objective means of selecting participants that prevents researcher bias being able to skew the sample. Further, because the sampling method is independent of any particular characteristic (besides the arbitrary characteristic of the participant’s order in the list) this sample is likely to be representative of the population as a whole.

Weaknesses of systematic sampling:

  • Unexpected bias: Some characteristics could occur more or less frequently at certain intervals, making a sample that is selected based on that interval biased. For example, houses tend to be have even numbers on one side of a road and odd numbers on the other. If one side of the road is more expensive than the other and you select every 4th house, say, then you will only select even numbers from one side of the road – and this sample may not be representative of the road as a whole.

Stratified sampling

The stratified sampling method involves dividing the population into relevant groups for study, working out what percentage of the population is in each group, and then randomly sampling the population according to these percentages.

For example, let’s say 20% of the population is aged 0-18, and 50% of the population is aged 19-65, and 30% of the population is aged >65. A stratified sample of 100 participants would randomly select 20x 0-18 year olds, 50x 19-65 year olds, and 30x people over 65.

Strengths of stratified sampling:

  • Representative: The stratification is deliberately designed to yield a sample that is representative of the population as a whole. You won’t get people with certain characteristics being over- or under-represented within the sample.
  • Unbiased: Because participants within each group are selected randomly , researcher bias is unable to skew who is included in the study.

Weaknesses of stratified sampling:

  • Requires knowledge of population breakdown: Researchers need to accurately gauge what percentage of the population falls into what group. If the researchers get these percentages wrong, the sample will be biased and some groups will be over- or under-represented.

Opportunity and volunteer sampling

The opportunity and volunteer sampling methods:

  • E.g. Approaching people in the street and asking them to complete a questionnaire.
  • E.g. Placing an advert online inviting people to complete a questionnaire.

Strengths of opportunity and volunteer sampling:

  • Quick and easy: Approaching participants ( opportunity sampling) or inviting participants ( volunteer sampling) is quick and straightforward. You don’t have to spend time compiling details of the target population (like in e.g. random or systematic sampling ), nor do you have to spend time dividing participants according to relevant categories (like in stratified sampling ).
  • May be the only option: With natural experiments – where a variable changes as a result of something outside the researchers’ control – opportunity sampling may be the only viable sampling method. For example, researchers couldn’t randomly sample 10 cities from all the cities in the world and change the drug laws in those cities to see the effects – they don’t have that kind of power. However, if a city is naturally changing its drug laws anyway, researchers could use opportunity sampling to study that city for research.

Weaknesses of opportunity and volunteer sampling:

  • Unrepresentative: The pool of participants will likely be biased towards certain kinds of people. For example, if you conduct opportunity sampling on a weekday at 10am, this sample will likely exclude people who are at work. Similarly, volunteer sampling is likely to exclude people who are too busy to take part in the study.

Independent vs. dependent variables

If the study involves an experiment , the researchers will alter an independent variable to measure its effects on a dependent variable :

  • E.g. In Bickman’s study of the effects of uniforms on obedience , the independent variable was the uniform of the person giving orders.
  • E.g. In Bickman’s study of the effects of uniforms on obedience , the dependent variable was how many people followed the orders.

Extraneous and confounding variables

In addition to the variables actually being investigated ( independent and dependent ), there may be additional (unwanted) variables in the experiment. These additional variables are called extraneous variables .

Researchers must control for extraneous variables to prevent them from skewing the results and leading to false conclusions. When extraneous variables are not properly controlled for they are known as confounding variables .

For example, if you’re studying the effect of caffeine on reaction times, it might make sense to conduct all experiments at the same time of day to prevent this extraneous variable from confounding the results. Reaction times change throughout the day and so if you test one group of subjects at 3pm and another group right before they go to bed, you may falsely conclude that the second group had slower reaction times.

Operationalisation of variables

Operationalisation of variables is where researchers clearly and measurably define the variables in their study.

For example, an experiment on the effects of sleep ( independent variable ) on anxiety ( dependent variable ) would need to clearly operationalise each variable. Sleep could be defined by number of hours spent in bed, but anxiety is a bit more abstract and so researchers would need to operationalise (i.e. define) anxiety such that it can be quantified in a measurable and objective way.

If variables are not properly operationalised, the experiment cannot be properly replicated , experimenters’ subjective interpretations may skew results, and the findings may not be valid .

Pilot studies

A pilot study is basically a practice run of the proposed research project. Researchers will use a small number of participants and run through the procedure with them. The purpose of this is to identify any problems or areas for improvement in the study design before conducting the research in full. A pilot study may also give an early indication of whether the results will be statistically significant .

For example, if a task is too easy for participants, or it’s too obvious what the real purpose of an experiment is, or questions in a questionnaire are ambiguous, then the results may not be valid . Conducting a pilot study first may save time and money as it enables researchers to identify and address such issues before conducting the full study on thousands of participants.

Study reporting

Features of a psychological report.

The report of a psychological study (research paper) typically contains the following sections in the following order:

  • Title: A short and clear description of the research.
  • Abstract: A summary of the research. This typically includes the aim and hypothesis , methods, results, and conclusion.
  • Introduction: Funnel technique: Broad overview of the context (e.g. current theories, previous studies, etc.) before focusing in on this particular study, why it was conducted, its aims and hypothesis .
  • Study design: This will explain what method was used (e.g. experiment or observation ), how the study was designed (e.g. independent groups or repeated measures ), and identification and operationalisation of variables .
  • Participants: A description of the target population to be studied, the sampling method , how many participants were included.
  • Equipment used: A description of any special equipment used in the study and how it was used.
  • Standardised procedure: A detailed step-by-step description of how the study was conducted. This allows for the study to be replicated by other researchers.
  • Controls : An explanation of how extraneous variables were controlled for so as to generate accurate results.
  • Results: A presentation of the key findings from the data collected. This is typically written summaries of the raw data ( descriptive statistics ), which may also be presented in tables , charts, graphs , etc. The raw data itself is typically included in appendices.
  • Discussion: An explanation of what the results mean and how they relate to the experimental hypothesis (supporting or contradicting it), any issues with how results were generated, how the results fit with other research, and suggestions for future research.
  • Conclusion: A short summary of the key findings from the study.
  • Book: Milgram, S., 2010. Obedience to Authority . 1st ed. Pinter & Martin.
  • Journal article: Bandura, A., Ross, D. and Ross, S., 1961. Transmission of Aggression through Imitation of Aggressive Models . The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), pp.575-582.
  • Appendices: This is where you put any supporting materials that are too detailed or long to include in the main report. For example, the raw data collected from a study, or the complete list of questions in a questionnaire .

Peer review

Peer review is a way of assessing the scientific credibility of a research paper before it is published in a scientific journal. The idea with peer review is to prevent false ideas and bad research from being accepted as fact.

It typically works as follows: The researchers submit their paper to the journal they want it to be published in, and the editor of that journal sends the paper to expert reviewers (i.e. psychologists who are experts in that area – the researchers’ ‘peers’) who evaluate the paper’s scientific validity. The reviewers may accept the paper as it is, accept it with a few changes, reject it and suggest revisions and resubmission at a later date, or reject it completely.

There are several different methods of peer review:

  • Open review: The researchers and the reviewers are known to each other.
  • Single-blind: The researchers do not know the names of the reviewers. This prevents the researchers from being able to influence the reviewer. This is the most common form of peer review.
  • Double-blind: The researchers do not know the names of the reviewers, and the reviewers do not know the names of the researchers. This additionally prevents the reviewer’s bias towards the researcher from influencing their decision whether to accept their paper or not.

Criticisms of peer review:

  • Bias: There are several ways peer review can be subject to bias. For example, academic research (particularly in niche areas) takes place among a fairly small circle of people who know each other and so these relationships may affect publication decisions. Further, many academics are funded by organisations and companies that may prefer certain ideas to be accepted as scientifically legitimate, and so this funding may produce conflicts of interest.
  • Doesn’t always prevent fraudulent/bad research from being published: There are many examples of fraudulent research passing peer review and being published (see this Wikipedia page for examples).
  • Prevents progress of new ideas: Reviewers of papers are typically older and established academics who have made their careers within the current scientific paradigm. As such, they may reject new or controversial ideas simply because they go against the current paradigm rather than because they are unscientific.
  • Plagiarism: In single-blind and double-blind peer reviews, the reviewer may use their anonymity to reject or delay a paper’s publication and steal the good ideas for themself.
  • Slow: Peer review can mean it takes months or even years between the researcher submitting a paper and its publication.

Study evaluation

In psychological studies, ethical issues are questions of what is morally right and wrong. An ethically-conducted study will protect the health and safety of the participants involved and uphold their dignity, privacy, and rights.

To provide guidance on this, the British Psychological Association has published a code of human research ethics :

  • Participants are told the project’s aims , the data being collected, and any risks associated with participation.
  • Participants have the right to withdraw or modify their consent at any time.
  • Researchers can use incentives (e.g. money) to encourage participation, but these incentives can’t be so big that they would compromise a participant’s freedom of choice.
  • Researchers must consider the participant’s ability to consent (e.g. age, mental ability, etc.)
  • Prior (general) consent: Informing participants that they will be deceived without telling them the nature of the deception. However, this may affect their behaviour as they try to guess the real nature of the study.
  • Retrospective consent: Informing participants that they were deceived after the study is completed and asking for their consent. The problem with this is that if they don’t consent then it’s too late.
  • Presumptive consent: Asking people who aren’t participating in the study if they would be willing to participate in the study. If these people would be willing to give consent, then it may be reasonable to assume that those taking part in the study would also give consent.
  • Confidentiality: Personal data obtained about participants should not be disclosed (unless the participant agreed to this in advance). Any data that is published will not be publicly identifiable as the participant’s.
  • Debriefing: Once data gathering is complete, researchers must explain all relevant details of the study to participants – especially if deception was involved. If a study might have harmed the individual (e.g. its purpose was to induce a negative mood), it is ethical for the debrief to address this harm (e.g. by inducing a happy mood) so that the participant does not leave the study in a worse state than when they entered.

Reliability

Study results are reliable if the same results can be consistently replicated under the same circumstances. If results are inconsistent then the study is unreliable.

Note: Just because a study is reliable, its results are not automatically valid . A broken tape measure may reliably (i.e. consistently) record a person’s height as 200m, but that doesn’t mean this measurement is accurate.

There are several ways researchers can assess a study’s reliability:

Test-retest

Test-retest is when you give the same test to the same person on two different occasions. If the results are the same or similar both times, this suggests they are reliable.

For example, if your study used scales to measure participants’ weight, you would expect the scales to record the same (or a very similar) weight for the same person in the morning as in the evening. If the scales said the person weighed 100kg more later that same day, the scales (and therefore the results of the study) would be unreliable.

Inter-observer

Inter-observer reliability is a way to test the reliability of observational studies .

For example, if your study required observers to assess participants’ anxiety levels, you would expect different observers to grade the same behaviour in the same way. If one observer rated a participant’s behaviour a 3 for anxiety, and another observer rated the exact same behaviour an 8, the results would be unreliable.

Inter-observer reliability can be assessed mathematically by looking for correlation between observers’ scores. Inter-observer reliability can be improved by setting clearly defined behavioural categories .

Study results are valid if they accurately measure what they are supposed to. There are several ways researchers can assess a study’s validity:

  • E.g. let’s say you come up with a new test to measure participants’ intelligence levels. If participants scoring highly on your test also scored highly on a standardised IQ test and vice versa, that would suggest your test has concurrent validity because participants’ scores are correlated with a known accurate test.
  • E.g. a study that measures participants’ intelligence levels by asking them when their birthday is would not have face validity. Getting participants to complete a standardised IQ test would have greater face validity.
  • E.g. let’s say your study was supposed to measure aggression levels in response to someone annoying. If the study was conducted in a lab and the participant knew they were taking part in a study, the results probably wouldn’t have much ecological validity because of the unrealistic environment.
  • E.g. a study conducted in 1920 that measured participants’ attitudes towards social issues may have low temporal validity because societal attitudes have changed since then.

Control of extraneous variables

There are several different types of extraneous variables that can reduce the validity of a study. A well-conducted psychological study will control for these extraneous variables so that they do not skew the results.

Demand characteristics

Demand characteristics are extraneous variables where the demands of a study make participants behave in ways they wouldn’t behave outside of the study. This reduces the study’s ecological validity .

For example, if a participant guesses the purpose of an experiment they are taking part in, they may try to please the researcher by behaving in the ‘right’ way rather than the way they would naturally. Alternatively, the participant might rebel against the study and deliberately try to sabotage it (e.g. by deliberately giving wrong answers).

In some study designs, researchers can control for demand characteristics using single- blind methods. For example, a drug trial could give half the participants the actual drug and the other half a placebo but not tell participants which treatment they received. This way, both groups will have equal demand characteristics and so any differences between them should be down to the drug itself.

Investigator effects

Investigator effects are another extraneous variable where the characteristics of the researcher affect the participant’s behaviour. Again, this reduces the study’s ecological validity .

Many characteristics – e.g. the researcher’s age, gender, accent, what they’re wearing – could potentially influence the participant’s responses. For example, in an interview about sex, females may feel less comfortable answering questions asked by a male interviewer and thus give different answers than if they were asked by a female. The researcher’s biases may also come across in their body language or tone of voice, affecting the participant’s responses.

In some study designs, researchers can control for demand characteristics using double- blind methods. In a double-blind drug trial, for example, neither the participants nor the researchers know which participants get the actual drug and which get the placebo. This way, the researcher is unable to give any clues (consciously or unconsciously) to participants that would affect their behaviour.

Participant variables

Participant variables are differences between participants. These can be controlled for by random allocation .

For example, in an experiment on the effect of caffeine on reaction times, participants would be randomly allocated into either the caffeine group or the non-caffeine group. A non -random allocation method, such as allocating caffeine to men and placebo to women, could mean variables in the allocation method (in this case gender) skew the results. When participants are randomly allocated, any extraneous variables (e.g. gender in this case) will be allocated evenly between each group and so not skew the results of one group more than the other.

Situational variables

Situational variables are the environment the experiment is conducted in. These can be controlled for by standardisation .

For example, all the tests of caffeine on reaction times would be conducted in the same room, at the same time of day, using the same equipment, and so on to prevent these features of the environment from skewing the results.

In a repeated measures experiment, researchers may use counterbalancing to control for the order in which tasks are completed.

For example, half of participants would do task A followed by task B, and the other half would do task B followed by task A.

Implications of psychological research for the economy

Psychological research often has practical applications in real life. The following are some examples of how psychological findings may affect the economy:

  • Attachment : Bowlby’s maternal deprivation hypothesis suggests that periods of extended separation between mother and child before age 3 are harmful to the child’s psychological development. And if mothers stay at home during this period, they can’t go out to work. However, some more recent research challenges Bowlby’s conclusions, suggesting that substitutes (e.g. the father , or nursery care) can care for the child, allowing the mother to go back to work sooner and remain economically active.
  • Depression : Psychological research has found effective therapies for treating depression, such as cognitive behavioural therapy and SSRIs. The benefits of such therapies – if they are effective – are likely to outweigh the costs because they enable the person to return to work and pay taxes, as well avoiding long-term costs to the health service.
  • OCD : Similar to above: Drug therapies (e.g. SSRIs) and behavioural approaches (e.g. CBT) may alleviate OCD symptoms, enabling OCD sufferers to return to work, pay taxes, and avoid reliance on healthcare services.
  • Memory : Public money is required to fund police investigations. Psychological tools, such as the cognitive interview , have improved the accuracy of eyewitness testimonies, which equates to more efficient use of police time and resources.

Features of science

Theory construction and hypothesis testing.

Science works by making empirical observations of the world, formulating hypotheses /theories that explain these observations, and repeatedly testing these hypotheses /theories via experimentation.

  • E.g. A tape measure provides a more objective measurement of something compared to a researcher’s guess. Similarly, a set of scales is a more objective way of determining which of two objects is heavier than a researcher lifting each up and giving their opinion.
  • E.g. Burger (2009) replicated Milgram’s experiments with similar results.
  • E.g. The hypothesis that “water boils at 100°c” could be falsified by an experiment where you heated water to 999°c and it didn’t boil. In contrast, “everything doubles in size every 10 seconds” could not be falsified by any experiment because whatever equipment you used to measure everything would also double in size.
  • Freud’s psychodynamic theories are often criticised for being unfalsifiable: There’s not really any observations that could disprove them because every possible behaviour (e.g. crying or not crying) could be explained as the result of some unconscious thought process.

Paradigm shifts

Philosopher Thomas Kuhn argues that science is not as unbiased and objective as it seems. Instead, the majority of scientists just accept the existing scientific theories (i.e. the existing paradigm) as true and then find data that supports these theories while ignoring/rejecting data that refutes them.

Rarely, though, minority voices are able to successfully challenge the existing paradigm and replace it with a new one. When this happens it is a paradigm shift . An example of a paradigm shift in science is that from Newtonian gravity to Einstein’s theory of general relativity.

Data handling and analysis

Types of data, quantitative vs. qualitative.

Data from studies can be quantitative or qualitative :

  • Quantitative: Numerical
  • Qualitative: Non-numerical

For example, some quantitative data in the Milgram experiment would be how many subjects delivered a lethal shock. In contrast, some qualitative data would be asking the subjects afterwards how they felt about delivering the lethal shock.

Strengths of quantitative data / weaknesses of qualitative data:

  • Can be compared mathematically and scientifically: Quantitative data enables researchers to mathematically and objectively analyse data. For example, mood ratings of 7 and 6 can be compared objectively, whereas qualitative assessments such as ‘sad’ and ‘unhappy’ are hard to compare scientifically.

Weaknesses of quantitative data / strengths of qualitative data:

  • Less detailed: In reducing data to numbers and narrow definitions, quantitative data may miss important details and context.

Content analysis

Although the detail of qualitative data may be valuable, this level of detail can also make it hard to objectively or mathematically analyse. Content analysis is a way of analysing qualitative data. The process is as follows:

  • E.g. A bunch of unstructured interviews on the topic of childhood
  • E.g. Discussion of traumatic events, happy memories, births, and deaths
  • E.g. Researchers listen to the unstructured interviews and count how often traumatic events are mentioned
  • Statistical analysis is carried out on this data

Primary vs. secondary

Researchers can produce primary data or use secondary data to achieve the research aims of their study:

  • Primary data: Original data collected for the study
  • Secondary data: Data from another study previously conducted

Meta-analysis

A meta-analysis is a study of studies. It involves taking several smaller studies within a certain research area and using statistics to identify similarities and trends within those studies to create a larger study.

We have looked at some examples of meta-analyses elsewhere in the course such as Van Ijzendoorn’s meta-analysis of several strange situation studies and Grootheest et al’s meta-analysis of twin studies on OCD .

A good meta-analysis is often more reliable than a regular study because it is based on a larger data set, and any issues with one single study will be balanced out by the other studies.

Descriptive statistics

Measures of central tendency: mean, median, mode.

Mean , median , and mode are measures of central tendency . In other words, they are ways of reducing large data sets into averages .

The mean is calculated by adding all the numbers in a set together and dividing the total by the number of numbers.

  • Example set: 22, 78, 3, 33, 90
  • 22+78+3+33+90=226
  • The mean is 45.2
  • Uses all data in the set.
  • Accurate: Provides a precise number based on all the data in a set.

Weaknesses:

  • E.g.: 1, 3, 2, 5, 9, 4, 913 <- the mean is 133.9, but the 913 could be a measurement error or something and thus the mean is not representative of the data set

The median is calculated by arranging all the numbers in a set from smallest to biggest and then finding the number in the middle. Note: If the total number of numbers is odd, you just pick the middle one. But if the total number of numbers is even, you take the mid-point between the two numbers in the middle.

  • Example set: 20, 66, 85, 45, 18, 13, 90, 28, 9
  • 9, 13, 18, 20, 28 , 45, 66, 85, 90
  • The median is 28
  • Won’t be skewed by freak scores (unlike the mean).
  • E.g.: 1, 1, 3 , 9865, 67914 <- 3 is not really representative of the larger numbers in the set.
  • Less accurate/sensitive than the mean.

The mode is calculated by counting which is the most commonly occurring number in a set.

  • Example set: 7, 7, 20 , 16, 1, 20 , 25, 16, 20 , 9
  • There are two 7’s, but three 20’s
  • The mode is 20
  • Makes more sense for presenting the central tendency in data sets with whole numbers. For example, the average number of limbs for a human being will have a mean of something like 3.99, but a mode of 4.
  • Does not use all the data in a set.
  • A data set may have more than one mode.

Measures of dispersion: Range and standard deviation

Range and standard deviation are measures of dispersion . In other words, they quantify how much scores in a data set vary .

The range is calculated by subtracting the smallest number in the data set from the largest number.

  • Example set: 59, 8, 7, 84, 9, 49, 14, 75, 88, 11
  • The largest number is 88
  • The smallest number is 7
  • The range is 81
  • Easy and quick to calculate: You just subtract one number from another
  • Accounts for freak scores (highest and lowest)
  • Can be skewed by freak scores: The difference between the biggest and smallest numbers can be skewed by a single anomalous result or error, which may give an exaggerated impression of the data distribution compared to standard deviation .
  • 4, 4, 5, 5, 5, 6, 6, 7, 19
  • 4, 16, 16, 17, 17, 17, 18, 19 19

Standard deviation

The standard deviation (σ) is a measure of how much numbers in a data set deviate from the mean (average). It is calculated as follows:

  • Example data set: 59, 79, 43, 42, 81, 100, 38, 54, 92, 62
  • Calculate the mean (65)
  • -6, 14, -22, -23, 16, 35, -27, -11, 27, -3
  • 36, 196, 484, 529, 256, 1225, 729, 121, 729, 9
  • 36+196+484+529+256+1225+729+121+729+9=4314
  • 4314/10=431.4
  • √431.4=20.77
  • The standard deviation is 20.77

Note: This method of standard deviation is based on the entire population. There is a slightly different method for calculating based on a sample where instead of dividing by the number of numbers in the second to last step, you divide by the number of numbers-1 (in this case 4314/9=479.333). This gives a standard deviation of 21.89.

  • Is less skewed by freak scores: Standard deviation measures the average difference from the mean and so is less likely to be skewed by a single freak score (compared to the range ).
  • Takes longer to calculate than the range .

Percentages

A percentage (%) describes how much out of 100 something occurs. It is calculated as follows:

  • Example: 63 out of a total of 82 participants passed the test
  • 63/82=0.768
  • 0.768*100=76.8
  • 76.8% of participants passed the test

Percentage change

To calculate a percentage change, work out the difference between the original number and the after number, divide that difference by the original number, then multiply the result by 100:

  • Example: He got 80 marks on the test but after studying he got 88 marks on the test
  • His test score increased by 10% after studying

Normal and skewed distributions

Normal distribution.

A data set that has a normal distribution will have the majority of scores on or near the mean average. A normal distribution is also symmetrical: There are an equal number of scores above the mean as below it. In a normal distribution, scores become rarer and rarer the more they deviate from the mean.

An example of a normal distribution is IQ scores. As you can see from the histogram below, there are as many IQ scores below the mean as there are above the mean :

statistical infrequency bell curve

When plotted on a histogram , data that follows a normal distribution will form a bell-shaped curve like the one above.

Skewed distribution

positive skew and negative skew histograms

Skewed distributions are caused by outliers: Freak scores that throw off the mean . Skewed distributions can be positive or negative :

  • Mean > Median > Mode
  • Mean < Median < Mode

Correlation

Correlation refers to how closely related two (or more) things are related. For example, hot weather and ice cream sales may be positively correlated: When hot weather goes up, so do ice cream sales.

Correlations are measured mathematically using correlation coefficients (r). A correlation coefficient will be anywhere between +1 and -1:

  • r=+1 means two things are perfectly positively correlated: When one goes up , so does the other by the same amount
  • r=-1 means two things perfectly negatively correlated: When one goes up , the other goes down by the same amount
  • r=0 means two things are not correlated at all: A change in one is totally independent of a change in the other

The following scattergrams illustrate various correlation coefficients:

correlation coefficient scatter graph examples

Presentation of data

table example

For example, the behavioural categories table above presents the raw data of each student in this made-up study. But in the results section, researchers might include another table that compares average anxiety rating scores for males and females.

Scattergrams

scattergram example

For example, each dot on the correlation scattergram opposite could represent a student. The x-axis could represent the number of hours the student studied, and the y-axis could represent the student’s test score.

eyewitness testimony loftus and palmer

For example, the results of Loftus and Palmer’s study into the effects of different leading questions on memory could be presented using the bar chart above. It’s not like there are categories in-between ‘contacted’ and ‘hit’, so the bars have gaps between them (unlike a histogram ).

A histogram is a bit like a bar chart but is used to illustrate continuous or interval data (rather than discrete data or whole numbers).

histogram example

Because the data on the x axis is continuous, there are no gaps between the bars.

line graph example

For example, the line graph above illustrates 3 different people’s progression in a strength training program over time.

pie chart example

For example, the frequency with which different attachment styles occurred in Ainsworth’s strange situation could be represented by the pie chart opposite.

Inferential testing

Probability and significance.

The point of inferential testing is to see whether a study’s results are statistically significant , i.e. whether any observed effects are as a result of whatever is being studied rather than just random chance.

For example, let’s say you are studying whether flipping a coin outdoors increases the likelihood of getting heads. You flip the coin 100 times and get 52 heads and 48 tails. Assuming a baseline expectation of 50:50, you might take these results to mean that flipping the coin outdoors does increase the likelihood of getting heads. However, from 100 coin flips, a ratio of 52:48 between heads and tails is not very significant and could have occurred due to luck. So, the probability that this difference in heads and tails is because you flipped the coin outside (rather than just luck) is low.

Probability is denoted by the symbol p . The lower the p value, the more statistically significant your results are. You can never get a p value of 0, though, so researchers will set a threshold at which point the results are considered statistically significant enough to reject the null hypothesis . In psychology, this threshold is usually <0.05, which means there is a less than 5% chance the observed effect is due to luck and a >95% chance it is a real effect.

Type 1 and type 2 errors

When interpreting statistical significance, there are two types of errors:

  • E.g. The p threshold is <0.05, but the researchers’ results are among the 5% of fluke outcomes that look significant but are just due to luck
  • E.g. The p threshold is set too low (e.g. <0.01), and the data falls short (e.g. p=<0.02)

Increasing the sample size reduces the likelihood of type 1 and type 2 errors.

Key maths skills made easy!

psychology research methods maths skills revision guide

Types of statistical test

Note: The inferential tests below are needed for A level only, if you are taking the AS exam , you only need to know the sign test .

There are several different types of inferential test in addition to the sign test . Which inferential test is best for a study will depend on the following three criteria:

  • Whether you are looking for a difference or a correlation
  • E.g. at the competition there were 8 runners, 12 swimmers, and 6 long jumpers (it’s not like there are in-between measurements between ‘swimmer’ and ‘runner’)
  • E.g. First, second, and third place in a race
  • E.g. Ranking your mood on a scale of 1-10
  • E.g. Weights in kg
  • E.g. Heights in cm
  • E.g. Times in seconds
  • Whether the experimental design is related (i.e. repeated measures ) or unrelated (i.e. independent groups )

The following table shows which inferential test is appropriate according to these criteria:

Mann Whitney
test test

Note: You won’t have to work out all these tests from scratch, but you may need to:

  • Say which of the statistical tests is appropriate (i.e. based on whether it’s a difference or correlation; whether the data is nominal, ordinal, or interval; and whether the data is related or unrelated).
  • Identify the critical value from a critical values table and use this to say whether a result (which will be given to you in the exam) is statistically significant.

The sign test

The sign test is a way to calculate the statistical significance of differences between related pairs (e.g. before and after in a repeated measures experiment ) of nominal data. If the observed value (s) is equal or less than the critical value (cv), the results are statistically significant.

Example: Let’s say we ran an experiment on 10 participants to see whether they prefer movie A or movie B .

A 3 6
B 3 3
C 5 6
D 4 6
E 2 3
F 3 7
G 5 3
H 7 8
I 2 6
J 8 5
  • n = 9 (because even though there are 10 participants, one participant had no change so we exclude them from our calculation)
  • In this case our experimental hypothesis is two-tailed: Participants may prefer movie A or movie B
  • (The null hypothesis is that participants like both movies equally)
  • In this case, let’s say it’s 0.1
  • The experimental hypothesis is two-tailed
  • So, in this example, our critical value (cv) is 1
  • In this example, there are 2 As, so our observed value (s) is 2
  • In this example, the observed value (2) is greater than the critical value (1) and so the results are not statistically significant. This means we must accept the null hypothesis and reject the experimental hypothesis .

<<<Biopsychology

A-Level AQA Psychology Questions by Topic

Finish sign up, filter by paper, core content, 1. social influence.

TopicMark Scheme
TopicMark Scheme

3. Attachment

TopicMark Scheme

4 . Psychopathology

TopicMark Scheme

5 . Approaches in Psychology

TopicMark Scheme

6. Biopsychology

7 . research methods, 8. issues and debates in psychology, 9. relationships.

TopicMark Scheme

11. Cognition and Development

TopicMark Scheme

12. Schizophrenia

13. eating behaviour, 15. aggression, 16. forensic psychology, 17. addiction.

100 Most Popular Courses For September

aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

Harvard and MIT’s $800 Million Mistake: The Triple Failure of 2U, edX, and Axim Collaborative

The future of Coursera’s only credible alternative for universities rests in the hands of 2U’s creditors.

  • [2024] 250 Top Domestika Courses of All Time
  • [2024] 150+ Psychology Courses You Can Take for Free
  • Layoffs Hit Udemy: 20% Out as Some Jobs Move Overseas
  • 7 Best Online First Aid Courses and Sites for 2024
  • 30% of Coursera Courses Have Peer Reviews: Here Are the Best

600 Free Google Certifications

Most common

Popular subjects.

Computer Science

Data Analysis

Cybersecurity

Popular courses

Tsinghua Chinese: Start Talking with 1.3 Billion People

Marketing Innovative Products and Services

Learning How to Learn: Powerful mental tools to help you master tough subjects

Organize and share your learning with Class Central Lists.

View our Lists Showcase

Class Central is learner-supported. When you buy through links on our site, we may earn an affiliate commission.

Research Methods - Psychology A-Level - AQA Paper 2

via YouTube Help

Types of Experiment - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Observation - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Self-report techniques - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Correlations - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Content analysis - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Case Studies - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Aims and Hypotheses - Research Methods [ A Level Psychology ]. Sampling - Research Methods [A-Level Psychology]. Experimental Design - Research Methods [A-Level Psychology]. Variables - Research Methods (7.13) Psychology AQA paper 2. Control of Extraneous Variables - Research Methods (7.14) Psychology AQA paper 2. Demand Characteristics - Research Methods (7.15) Psychology AQA paper 2. Ethics - Research Methods (7.16) Psychology AQA paper 2. Peer review - Research Methods (7.17) Psychology AQA paper 2. Psychology and the Economy - Research Methods (7.18) Psychology AQA paper 2. Reliability - Research Methods (7.19) Psychology AQA paper 2. Validity - Research Methods (7.20) Psychology AQA paper 2. Features of Science - Research Methods (7.21) Psychology AQA paper 2. Reporting Psychological Studies - Research Methods (7.22) Psychology AQA paper 2. Quantitative and Qualitative - Research Methods (7.23) Psychology AQA paper 2. Primary & secondary data, meta analysis - Research Methods (7.24) Psychology AQA paper 2. Descriptive Statistics - Research Methods (7.25) Psychology AQA paper 2. Graphs - Research Methods (7.26) Psychology AQA paper 2. Distribution - Research Methods (7.27) Psychology AQA paper 2. Referencing - Research Methods (7.28) Psychology AQA paper 2. Sign Test - Research Methods (7.29) Psychology AQA paper 2. Probability and Significance - Research Methods (7.30) Psychology AQA paper 2. Levels of Measurement - Research Methods (7.31) Psychology AQA paper 2. Inferential Statistical Tests, - Research Methods (7.32) Psychology AQA paper 2. Design a Study - Research Methods (7.33) Psychology AQA paper 2.

Psych Boost

Related Courses

Psychology 105: research methods in psychology, qualitative research methods in psychology, research methods in psychology: certificate program, research methods in psychology: homework help resource, research methods in psychology for teachers: professional development, research methods in psychology: help and review, related articles, 150+ online classes can help you become a better parent, 100+ psychology courses you can take for free, from data to insights: 10 best data analysis courses for 2024.

Select rating

Start your review of Research Methods - Psychology A-Level - AQA Paper 2

Never Stop Learning.

Get personalized course recommendations, track subjects and courses with reminders, and more.

aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

Reference Library

Collections

  • See what's new
  • All Resources
  • Student Resources
  • Assessment Resources
  • Teaching Resources
  • CPD Courses
  • Livestreams

Study notes, videos, interactive activities and more!

Psychology news, insights and enrichment

Currated collections of free resources

Browse resources by topic

  • All Psychology Resources

Resource Selections

Currated lists of resources

Research Methods

Research Methods are the different tools/methods psychologists used to conduct psychological research, analyse data and draw conclusions.

  • Share on Facebook
  • Share on Twitter
  • Share by Email

Teaching Activity | Research Methods Pyramid

Teaching Activities

Cracking Confounding Variables | Teaching Activity

Psyched out: teaching activity.

aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

Psychology In The News | Deja Vu, or Jamais vu?

2nd January 2024

2023 Christmas Quizzes | AQA A Level Psychology

Weekly revision planner for ocr gcse psychology | 2024 exams.

Exam Support

Halloween Quiz 2023 | AQA A Level Psychology

Data skills shorts: teaching activity for aqa gcse psychology.

Practice Exam Questions

Research Methods Teaching Activity: Mix a Method

Ib psychology teaching activity: test research methods understanding, visualise this: using technology to model research method answers.

27th January 2017

Guess The Correlation Game

16th February 2017

Amusing Graphs ‘Prove’ That Correlation Does Not Imply Causation

20th February 2017

Q&A from AQA: Calculating Statistical Tests

24th February 2017

Q&A from AQA: The Sign Test & Significance

Q&a from aqa: standard deviation, q&a from aqa: levels of measurement, q&a from aqa: parametric vs. non-parametric tests, four new summer psychology cpd events.

4th April 2017

Example Answers for Research Methods: A Level Psychology, Paper 2, June 2019 (AQA)

Research methods: mcq revision test 2 for aqa a level psychology.

Quizzes & Activities

Research Methods: MCQ Revision Test 1 for AQA A Level Psychology

Topic Videos

Example Answer for Question 19 Paper 2: AS Psychology, June 2017 (AQA)

Example answers for research methods: a level psychology, paper 2, june 2018 (aqa), ​example answer for question 21 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), ​example answer for question 20 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), ​example answer for question 19 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 18 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 17 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 16 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 15 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 14 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 13 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), ​example answer for question 12 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 11 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), ​example answer for question 10 paper 2: a level psychology, june 2017 (aqa), essays matter. full stop. five reasons to order the aqa a level psychology topic essays..

4th September 2017

Psychology Revision Webinars Are Now Available On-Demand

9th June 2017

Three Essential Paper 2 Research Method Videos for A Level Psychology Students

Design a study: example answer video for a level sam 2, paper 2, q21 (9 marks), research methods: as webinar video 2016, case studies: example answer video for a level sam 3, paper 1, q4 (5 marks), research methods: as exam 2016 feedback video, example answer for question 22 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 21 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 20 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 18 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 17 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 16 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 15 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), example answer for question 14 paper 2: as psychology, june 2017 (aqa), model answer for question 11 paper 2: as psychology, june 2016 (aqa), model answer for question 3 paper 1: as psychology, june 2016 (aqa), a level psychology topic quiz - research methods, our subjects.

  • › Criminology
  • › Economics
  • › Geography
  • › Health & Social Care
  • › Psychology
  • › Sociology
  • › Teaching & learning resources
  • › Student revision workshops
  • › Online student courses
  • › CPD for teachers
  • › Livestreams
  • › Teaching jobs

Boston House, 214 High Street, Boston Spa, West Yorkshire, LS23 6AD Tel: 01937 848885

  • › Contact us
  • › Terms of use
  • › Privacy & cookies

© 2002-2024 Tutor2u Limited. Company Reg no: 04489574. VAT reg no 816865400.

This website works best with JavaScript switched on. Please enable JavaScript

  • Centre Services
  • Associate Extranet
  • All About Maths

AS and A-level Psychology

  • Specification
  • Planning resources
  • Teaching resources
  • Assessment resources
  • Introduction

Specification at a glance

  • 3.1 Introductory topics in Psychology
  • 3.2 Psychology in context
  • 4.1 Introductory topics in Psychology
  • 4.2 Psychology in context
  • 4.3 Issues and options in Psychology
  • Scheme of assessment
  • General administration
  • Mathematical requirements and exemplification

AS and A-level Specification at a glance

These qualifications are linear. Linear means that students will sit all the AS exams at the end of their AS course and all the A-level exams at the end of their A-level course.

Subject content

  • 1 Social influence
  • 3 Attachment
  • 4 Approaches in Psychology
  • 5 Psychopathology
  • 6 Research methods

Assessments

Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology

Compulsory content 1–3 above

Section A: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks

Section B: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks

Section C: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks

aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

Paper 2: Psychology in Context

Compulsory content 4–6 above

Section A: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks

Section B: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks

Section C: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks

  • 4 Psychopathology
  • 5 Approaches in Psychology
  • 6 Biopsychology
  • 7 Research methods
  • 8 Issues and debates in Psychology
  • 9 Relationships
  • 11 Cognition and development
  • 12 Schizophrenia
  • 13 Eating behaviour
  • 15 Aggression
  • 16 Forensic Psychology
  • 17 Addiction
Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology

Compulsory content 1–4 above

Paper 2: Psychology in Context

Compulsory content 5–7 above

Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology

Compulsory content 8 above

Optional content, one from option 1, 9–11, one from option 2, 12–14, one from option 3, 15–17 above

IMAGES

  1. AQA Paper 2 Psychology Revision Notes

    aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

  2. UNIT 2 PSYCHOLOGY AQA REVISION GUIDE (RESEARCH METHODS) DETAILED

    aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

  3. A-Level AQA Psychology

    aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

  4. AQA A-level PSYCHOLOGY7182/2 Paper 2 Psychology in context Question

    aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

  5. SOLUTION: Aqa 2022 a level psychology paper 2 markscheme

    aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

  6. Reserach methods checklist

    aqa a level psychology paper 2 research methods

VIDEO

  1. AQA A-Level Psychology

  2. A-Level Psychology (AQA): Research Methods

  3. ✨ AQA A-Level Psychology Paper 2 Predictions! ✨ #aqaalevelpsychology #aqaalevelpsychologypaper2 #pap

  4. A Level Psychology Paper 4 Exam Format 2024

  5. Edexcel GCSE Psychology (9-1)

  6. Top 5 Studies to Learn for Paper 2

COMMENTS

  1. AQA A-Level Psychology Past Papers With Answers

    The past papers are free to download for you to use as practice for your exams. Paper 1: Introductory Topics. Paper 2: Psychology in Context. Paper 3: Issues and Options. AS Psychology (7181): Paper 1. A-Level Psychology (7182): Paper 1. 72 Marks. 96 Marks. 90 minutes.

  2. AQA Psychology A-level: Research Methods Revision- PMT

    University of Cambridge - BA Natural Sciences (specialised in Psychology) Cambridge Graduate with Top5 Cohort Rank & Scholarship | 100% Success Rate for Top 20 Unis | 40% Students got Oxbridge Offer. This topic is included in A-level Papers 1 and 2 for AQA Psychology.

  3. Paper 2

    Paper 2 - Research Methods Paper 2 Research Methods AS/A Level Revision Notes (AQA) BACK TO STUDENT RESOURCES Research Methods Resources Quantitative And Qualitative Data: The Distinction Between Qualitative And Quantitative Data Collection Techniques Paper 2 - Psychology in Context | Research Methods | 30 Minutes Variables In Psychological Research Paper 2 - Psychology in […]

  4. AQA A-level Psychology Revision

    Revision for AQA Psychology AS and A-Level Papers, including past papers, videos, and summary notes. Revision for AQA Psychology AS and A-Level Papers, including past papers, videos, and summary notes. ... Research Methods. A-Level Paper 1. Topic 1: Social Influence. Topic 2: Memory. Topic 3: Attachment. Topic 4: Psychopathology. Topic 7 ...

  5. AS and A-level

    AQA Education intends to apply for an injunction preventing interference with public examinations. This notice is to alert you to the application and the proposed injunction, so that you are aware of it and can make submissions about it if you wish to do so. ... Question paper (A-level): Paper 2 Psychology in context - June 2022 Published 14 ...

  6. AQA A-Level Psychology Past Papers

    Scroll down to find papers from previous years. June 2023 - AQA A-Level Psychology (7182) Past Papers. A-Level Psychology Paper 1: Introductory Topics in Psychology (7182/1) Download Past Paper - Download Marking Scheme. A-Level Psychology Paper 2: Psychology in Context (7182/2) Download Past Paper - Download Marking Scheme.

  7. A-level Psychology AQA Revision Notes

    A-level: written exam: 2 hours: 96 marks in total: 33.3% of A-level; 5. Approaches to Psychology (24 marks) 6. Biopsychology (24 marks) 7. Research Methods (AS: 24 marks & A-level: 48 marks) multiple choice, short answer and extended writing Paper 3: Issues and Options in Psychology. written exam: 2 hours: 96 marks in total: 33.3% of A-level ...

  8. Research Methods

    Overview - Research Methods. Research methods are how psychologists and scientists come up with and test their theories. The A level psychology syllabus covers several different types of studies and experiments used in psychology as well as how these studies are conducted and reported:. Types of psychological studies (including experiments, observations, self-reporting, and case studies)

  9. Example Answers for Research Methods: A Level Psychology, Paper 2, June

    Here are some example answers to the written Paper 2 questions on Research Methods in the 2019 AQA exams. tutor2u. Main menu. Main menu Close panel. Home; Subjects ... Example Answer for Question 13 Paper 2: A Level Psychology, June 2017 (AQA) Exam Support. Mental bias can leave us unprepared for disasters 16th April 2020 ...

  10. AQA Psychology Paper 2

    1. classify population into sub groups. 2. choose a sample which consists of participants from each category in the same proportion as they are in the population. 3. then PPs are selected using random sampling. pros and cons of stratified sampling.

  11. A-Level AQA Psychology Questions by Topic

    15. Aggression. 16. Forensic Psychology. 17. Addiction. A-Level Psychology past paper questions by topic for AQA. Also offering past papers and videos for Edexcel and OCR.

  12. AQA A-Level Psychology: Paper 2

    Hope you guys enjoy this videoTopics not included: tests of correlation, parametric tests of difference, non-parametric tests of difference, and the chi-squa...

  13. AS and A-level

    4. Mark schemes. Question papers. Showing 62 results for research methods. Reset search. Question paper (Modified A4 18pt) (A-level): Paper 2 Psychology in context - June 2022. Published 14 Jul 2023 | PDF | 303 KB. Question paper (Modified A3 36pt) (A-level): Paper 2 Psychology in context - June 2022.

  14. Revision Help: Research Methods for A-Level Psychology

    Company Reg no: 04489574. VAT reg no 816865400. Use these revision materials to help with your studies of Research Methods for A-Level Psychology.

  15. PDF Question paper (A-level) : Paper 2 Psychology in context

    The marks for questions are shown in brackets. The maximum mark for this paper is 96. Questions should be answered in continuous prose. You will be assessed on your ability to: - use good English. - organise information clearly. - use specialist vocabulary where appropriate. IB/G/Jun22/E8. 7182/2.

  16. Research Methods

    Dive into a comprehensive video series covering Research Methods for AQA A-level Psychology Paper 2. Explore various research techniques, including types of experiments, observations, self-report methods, correlations, content analysis, and case studies.

  17. Research & The Economy

    CIE. Spanish Language & Literature. Past Papers. Other Subjects. Revision notes on 7.2.13 Research & The Economy for the AQA A Level Psychology syllabus, written by the Psychology experts at Save My Exams.

  18. Research Methods

    Example Answers for Research Methods: A Level Psychology, Paper 2, June 2018 (AQA) Exam Support. Example Answer for Question 21 Paper 2: A Level Psychology, June 2017 (AQA) Exam Support. Example Answer for Question 20 Paper 2: A Level Psychology, June 2017 (AQA) Exam Support. Example Answer for Question 19 Paper 2: A Level Psychology, June 2017 ...

  19. A Level Psychology (AQA)

    A research method or technique that involves a face-to-face, 'real-time' interaction with another individual and results in the collection of data. Interviewer bias. The effect of an interviewer's expectations, communicated unconsciously, on a respondent's behaviour. Questionnaire.

  20. AQA A-level psychology

    2) Validate quality & relevance of research - assessing hypothesis, methodology, statistical tests & conclusion for accuracy & quality. 3) Suggest amendments & improvements - extreme circumstances may lead to research being rejected sue to it being inappropriate for publication. AQA A-level psychology paper 2 research methods revision - June ...

  21. Subject content

    4.2 Psychology in context. Students will be expected to: demonstrate knowledge and understanding of psychological concepts, theories, research studies, research methods and ethical issues in relation to the specified Paper 2 content. apply psychological knowledge and understanding of the specified Paper 2 content in a range of contexts.

  22. AS and A-level

    Assessed. written exam: 1 hour 30 minutes. 72 marks in total. 50% of AS. Questions. Section A: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks. Section B: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks. Section C: multiple choice, short answer and extended writing, 24 marks. Paper 2: Psychology in Context.