• Skip to Content
  • Skip to Main Navigation
  • Skip to Search

research areas in cognitive psychology

Indiana University Bloomington Indiana University Bloomington IU Bloomington

Open Search

The College of Arts & Sciences

  • Department of Psychological and Brain Sciences
  • Student Portal
  • Research Areas

Cognitive Psychology

Research in cognitive psychology.

Cognitive psychologists develop a systematic understanding of cognitive processes. These include perception, human learning, attention, categorization, problem solving, decision–making, information processing and retrieval, short and long-term memory and forgetting, sensory encoding, motor control, psycholinguistics, and reading.

  • Information processing in decision making
  • Categorization and conceptual representation
  • Language acquisition and sentence processing
  • Speech perception
  • Analysis of choice behavior
  • Visual perception
  • Stages of sensory information processing
  • The focus of attention
  • Forgetting in short and long-term memory
  • The nature of retrieval from memory
  • The mechanisms of visual and auditory imagery
  • The control of the memorial system
  • Shared cognitive processing

Adaptive Behavior and Cognition Lab  - Peter Todd

  • Faculty + Staff Intranet

Psychological and Brain Sciences social media channels

  • College of Arts & Sciences

research areas in cognitive psychology

  • Clinical Psychological Science B.S.
  • Neuroscience B.S.
  • Organizational + Business Psychology B.S.
  • Psychology B.A.
  • Psychology B.S.
  • Psychology Minor
  • Neuroscience Certificate
  • Areas of Interest
  • Academic Support
  • Research Opportunities
  • Service-Learning + Practica
  • Volunteer Opportunities
  • Tutoring + Teaching Internships
  • Internships
  • Applying to Graduate School
  • Financing Graduate School
  • Honors Program
  • Ph.D. Degree
  • Qualifying Exams
  • Dissertation
  • Research Project
  • Double Major
  • Ph.D. Minor
  • Funding Opportunities
  • Meeting Room Reservations
  • Physical Plant Service Requests
  • Poster Printing
  • Undergraduate Teaching Assistant Assignments
  • Participate in Experiments

woman thinking

Photo by Alonso Reyes on Unsplash

Faculty in the Cognitive area investigate a wide range of topics including language, meaning and mental representation, perception, learning, memory, decision making and reasoning. Our methods include behavioral experimentation, computational and mathematical modeling, naturalistic observation and cross-cultural fieldwork, and neuro-imaging. We collaborate with neurophysiologists, neuropsychologists, developmental psychologists, linguists, and anthropologists. Insights and methods from Cognitive Psychology are widely applied in fields such as law, medicine, engineering and design, business, politics, and education.

Herbert Clark

Herbert Clark

  • Meaning & Mental Representation
  • Words & Categories

Judith Ellen Fan

Judith Ellen Fan

  • Computational Approaches
  • Developmental Approaches
  • Learning & Memory
  • Neuroimaging
  • Plasticity & Change
  • Reasoning & Problem Solving
  • Vision Science

Michael Frank

Michael Frank

  • Social Cognition

Justin Gardner

Justin Gardner

  • Decision Science

Tobias Gerstenberg

Tobias Gerstenberg

Noah Goodman

Noah Goodman

Kalanit Grill-Spector

Kalanit Grill-Spector

  • Psychopathology & Risk

Laura Gwilliams

Laura Gwilliams

Ellen Markman

Ellen Markman

Jay McClelland

Jay McClelland

  • Mathematical Cognition

Russell Poldrack

Russell Poldrack

  • Addictive Behaviors
  • Motivation & Emotion
  • Self-Regulation & Control

Anthony Wagner

Anthony Wagner

Phd students.

Adani Abutto

Adani Abutto

  • Self & Identity

grey Stanford tree

Sean Anderson

Aaron Beller

Aaron Beller

Veronica Boyce

Veronica Boyce

Satchel Grant

Satchel Grant

Jerome Han

Yuxuan (Effie) Li

Andrew (Joo Hun) Nam

Andrew (Joo Hun) Nam

Ben Prystawski

Ben Prystawski

David Rose

Daniel Wurgaft

Justin Yang

Justin Yang

Postdoctoral scholars.

Michael I. Demidenko

Michael I. Demidenko

Lara kirfel, research staff.

Logan Bennett

Logan Bennett

Patrick Bissett

Patrick Bissett

  • Interventions

Annabel Bouwer

Annabel Bouwer

Mika braginsky.

Joshua William Buckholtz

Joshua William Buckholtz

Rachel Calcott

Rachel Calcott

  • Stereotyping & Stereotype Threat

Gloria Cheng

Gloria Cheng

George earle kachergis.

Marianne Lacsamana

Marianne Lacsamana

Amy Lightbody Kuhns

Amy Lightbody Kuhns

Jeanette Alane Mumford

Jeanette Alane Mumford

Khanh K Nguyen

Khanh K Nguyen

Alfredo Ornelas

Alfredo Ornelas

Julia E. Rathmann-Bloch

Julia E. Rathmann-Bloch

Jaime Ali Holguin Rios

Jaime Ali Holguin Rios

Derek Rosenzweig

Derek Rosenzweig

Sunjae Shim

Sunjae Shim

Beau sievers.

Robert Zane Sparks

Robert Zane Sparks

Arnav Verma

Arnav Verma

Katherine hermann.

American Psychological Association Logo

Cognitive Psychology Explores Our Mental Processes

Brain Science and Cognitive Psychology Explores Our Mental Processes

Understanding Brain Science and Cognitive Psychology

The human brain is an amazing and powerful tool. It allows us to learn, see, remember, hear, perceive, understand and create language. Sometimes, the human brain also fails us.

Cognitive psychologists study how people acquire, perceive, process and store information. This work can range from exploring how we learn language to understanding the interplay between cognition and emotion.

New technologies like magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) allow researchers to see a picture of the brain at work — helping them to understand how a brain reacts to a particular stimulus or how differences in brain structure can affect a person’s health, personality or cognitive functioning.

Brain Science and Cognitive Psychology Applied

Brain science and cognitive psychology is one of the most versatile psychological specialty areas today — and one of the most in demand. All professions have a compelling interest in how the brain works. Educators, curriculum designers, engineers, scientists, judges, public health and safety officials, architects and graphic designers all want to know more about how the brain processes information. Their research and its resulting applications have become an integral part of how organizations, schools and businesses function and succeed. In clinical settings, cognitive psychologists seek to treat issues related to human mental processes, including Alzheimer’s disease, speech issues, memory loss and sensory or perception difficulties.

Pursuing a Career in Brain Science & Cognitive Psychology

Related books

Child Development at the Intersection ...

Clinical Neuropsychology

Essentials of Conditioning and Learning 5th

APA Handbook of Neuropsychology

Department Of Psychology and Neuroscience

Our Commitment to Research

Our faculty ranks include accomplished researchers who have made substantial contributions to the advancement of science in our field. The Department of Psychology and Neuroscience is consistently ranked in the top four departments in the College in terms of the external grant funding that is generated by the faculty and we were recently ranked 1st in the National Science Foundation survey for total Research and Development Funding. Read more about our reputation and accomplishments .

Developmental Research

Behavioral and Integrative Neuroscience

The primary focus of Behavioral and Integrative Neuroscience is to understand the biological basis of behavior. A central research theme in our program is to understand biological mechanisms underlying drug addiction including neural mechanisms mediating drug-seeking behaviors, pharmacological mechanisms underlying the analgesic and rewarding properties of abused substances, and the effects of abused substances on the immune system. In this regard, faculty conduct research using state-of-the-art experimental approaches including sophisticated behavioral assays, molecular and cell biology techniques, neuroimaging, electrophysiology, electrochemistry, and molecular genetic tools. Read more about our faculty’s research interests .

Clinical Psychology

Faculty research in Clinical Psychology encompasses a wide range of topics, ranging from the causes of psychological disorders and the psychological mechanisms underlying symptoms, to applied domains involving the development and evaluation of intervention and assessment instruments. Several major themes emerge in faculty research, including the study of the nature of psychopathology, the effectiveness of psychotherapy interventions, prevention, and therapy, health psychology, interpersonal relationships, and the study of ethnic minority issues related to psychological health. Read more about our faculty’s research interests .

Cognitive Psychology

Cognitive Psychology is the scientific study of mental processes underlying behavior, a broad research area encompassing the study of attention, perception, memory, language, reasoning and problem-solving. These research areas are strongly represented by the faculty of the Cognitive Psychology Program along with research on cognitive aging and cognitive neuroscience. The primary research method is behavioral experimentation with humans (both college-aged adults and healthy older adults). Other important methods use neuroimaging technologies (such as fMRI and ERP), which allow us to correlate neural processing with cognitive function, eye-tracking technologies, which allow for the study of reading and visual perception, and experimentation with special neurological populations, whose cognitive deficits can inform our understanding of the effects of brain damage on normal cognition. Read more about our faculty’s research interests .

Developmental Psychology

Faculty in the Developmental Psychology Program are interested in many of the same cognitive and social phenomena as our colleagues in other Programs, but with a specific focus on how these phenomena emerge, stabilize, and change as a function of maturation and experience. Thus, we conduct research on infancy, childhood, and adolescence, using either cross-sectional designs in which age is an independent variable or longitudinal designs in which individuals are assessed over days, months, years, or generations. We imbed our research within the physiological context of neural, hormonal, and physical maturation, and within environmental contexts such as the parent-child dyad, the family, the classroom, and the community. Read more about our faculty’s research interests .

Quantitative Psychology

Internationally known for research and training in quantitative and statistical methods for psychological research, our Quantitative Psychology faculty have methodological interests in measurement theory, survey methods, and methods for analysis of correlational data. Our methodological research focuses on education diversity, structure of personality, issues in health psychology, statistical techniques for the study of change over time, and development and adaptation of modeling and analysis tools that are suited to evaluating linear and nonlinear dynamical systems models. All of our faculty are active in interdisciplinary substantive research in a variety of fields, including educational testing, substance abuse, child development, developmental psychopathology, and diversity in education. In this work, these faculty bring their quantitative and substantive expertise to enhance design and analysis of empirical research projects. Read more about our faculty’s research interests .

Social Psychology

Faculty research in Social Psychology encompass many core areas of the field, with a particular strength in affective processes as it relates to well-being, attitudes and stereotyping, and social judgment and decision-making. Faculty research interests are broad and include attitudes and attitude change, decision making, social cognition, emotions, stereotyping and prejudice, interpersonal processes, and group interactions. Read more about our faculty’s research interests .

Department of Psychology

You are here, cognitive psychology.

The program in Cognitive Psychology is concerned with basic research in perception, human learning and memory, consciousness, thinking and problem solving, language, and intelligence, as well as with applications of this basic research to everyday settings. Much of the research in the cognitive group bridges across these various fields of specialization. In addition to the core faculty in Cognitive Psychology, many faculty members with other primary specializations are also interested in cognitive research. Students are encouraged to read widely both in the cognitive area and in related disciplines, such as computer science, linguistics, philosophy, and statistics. Programs of study overlapping with other areas of psychological research such as developmental, social, clinical or neuroscience, are encouraged. The cognitive group meets together as a whole every Tuesday for a presentation (with discussion) by one of its members, and numerous smaller groups with special interests also meet on a regular basis for internal exchanges of ideas. Students are encouraged to study and do research with multiple faculty members, and to develop their own program of research in an area of their choosing. The Cognitive Psychology group is highly interactive and encourages research and discussion representing diverse viewpoints within the field.

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications Cover Image

Call for Papers: Automation and Human Cognition

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications is  calling for papers on Automation and Human Cognition.

Co-organizers: Amy S. McDonnell, University of Utah, USA ( [email protected] ) John D. Lee, University of Wisconsin-Madison ( [email protected] ) David L. Strayer, University of Utah, USA ( [email protected] )

Deadline/Submission Details Submission is open and papers should be submitted by October 1, 2023.

Click the title for more information.

Psychonomic Society Member Discount

Active Psychonomic Society Members are eligible for a discount on the APC for full-length articles. Please contact the Society for more information. 

Authors must submit the account code during the initial submission process. This account code can be entered under the "I believe I am covered by an institutional membership" payment option.

  • Most accessed

A one-man bilingual cocktail party: linguistic and non-linguistic effects on bilinguals’ speech recognition in Mandarin and English

Authors: Erin D. Smith, Lori L. Holt and Frederic Dick

Column setting and text justification influence return-sweep eye movement behavior during Chinese multi-line reading

Authors: Mengsi Wang, Donna E. Gill, Jeannie Judge, Chuanli Zang, Xuejun Bai and Simon P. Liversedge

How do students reason about statistical sampling with computer simulations? An integrative review from a grounded cognition perspective

Authors: Sebahat Gok and Robert L. Goldstone

Seeing the truck, but missing the cyclist: effects of blur on duration thresholds for road hazard detection

Authors: Silvia Guidi, Anna Kosovicheva and Benjamin Wolfe

Boosting wisdom of the crowd for medical image annotation using training performance and task features

Authors: Eeshan Hasan, Erik Duhaime and Jennifer S. Trueblood

Most recent articles RSS

View all articles

Teaching the science of learning

Authors: Yana Weinstein, Christopher R. Madan and Megan A. Sumeracki

Senses of place: architectural design for the multisensory mind

Authors: Charles Spence

Systemic racism: individuals and interactions, institutions and society

Authors: Mahzarin R. Banaji, Susan T. Fiske and Douglas S. Massey

Why does peer instruction benefit student learning?

Authors: Jonathan G. Tullis and Robert L. Goldstone

The effects of repetition frequency on the illusory truth effect

Authors: Aumyo Hassan and Sarah J. Barber

Most accessed articles RSS

Research in Progress at CRPI

CRPI welcomes submissions of Registered Reports  and also invites authors to make their protocols available in a citable, shareable and discoverable manner using our dedicated repository on figshare .

See below to keep up-to-date with the latest preregistered protocols that have been offered in-principle acceptance at the journal. 

Effects of roadside memorials on drivers’ risk perception and eye movements (Registered Report, Stage 1)

04 July 2019

Disgust and the rubber hand illusion: A registered replication report of Jalal, Krishnakumar, and Ramachandran (2015) (Registered Report, Stage 1)

15 May 2018

Related Journals

Psychonomic Bulletin & Review

Memory & Cognition

Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics

Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience

Learning & Behavior

Behavior Research Methods

Aims and scope

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications publishes new empirical and theoretical work covering all areas of Cognition, with a special emphasis on use-inspired basic research: fundamental research that grows from hypotheses about real-world problems. We expect that authors will be able to explain in a Significance section how their basic research serves to advance our understanding of the cognitive aspects of a problem with real-world applications.

The Cognitive Science of Medical Expertise

This special issue aims to bring together articles on the cognitive science of medical expertise. Studies in this special issue may examine learning by physicians and other medical professionals anywhere in the continuum from undergraduate study to continuing education. They may also probe the cognitive representation and processing of medical knowledge with studies of clinical reasoning or of medical errors. Studies may report novel empirical data and/or theoretical perspectives or reviews.

Submission Deadline:  December 31, 2021 Guest Editors:  Scott Fraundorf, Susanne Lajoie, Nikki Woods

Face Coverings: Considering the implications for face perception and speech communication

The proposed thematic series will highlight new work that characterizes the consequences of face masks on (a) the recognition and interpretation of facial expressions and emotions, (b) communication and social interactions, and (c) human and computational identity recognition and disguise. These will be addressed within the broad context of ways in which face perception and communication may change, comparisons of the social impact of face coverings in societies in which they are common versus those in which they are a new phenomenon, and changes in perceived interpersonal communication.

The overall goal is to develop accounts of how and why face coverings influence our face perception and speech communication, with specific attention to the relevant cognitive and behavioural mechanisms, as well as the practical implications and limitations.

Organized by: Karen Lander and Gabrielle Saunders Deadline for Submissions:   October 15, 2021

Systemic Racism: Cognitive Consequences and Interventions

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications (CRPI) publishes “use-inspired basic research”: fundamental and theoretically relevant research that grows from hypotheses about real-world problems. Systemic Racism is an urgent, real-world problem with implications for every area of cognitive research. The purpose of this special issue is to add to the scientific understanding of the cognitive roots of and fallout from minority status, discrimination, police violence, vigilantism, implicit bias, and more.

Organized by:  Jeremy M. Wolfe, Jennifer Gutsell, and Elizabeth Page-Gould Deadline for Submissions:  December 31, 2020

  • Editorial Board
  • Sign up for article alerts and news from this journal
  • 2023 Reviewer Acknowledgements
  • 2023 Psychonomic Society Best Article Award

Affiliated with

Psychonomic Society logo

Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications is affiliated with The Psychonomic Society

  • ISSN: 2365-7464 (electronic)

Annual Journal Metrics

2022 Citation Impact 4.1 - 2-year Impact Factor 4.1 - 5-year Impact Factor 1.383 - SNIP (Source Normalized Impact per Paper) 1.004 - SJR (SCImago Journal Rank)

2023 Speed 10 days submission to first editorial decision for all manuscripts (Median) 206 days submission to accept (Median)

2023 Usage  1,160,777 downloads 9,403 Altmetric mentions

  • More about our metrics

UCLA Department of Psychology

Cognitive Psychology

Information about the Cognitive Psychology Graduate Major

This Area’s goal is to produce cognitive psychologists who combine general scholarship with sophistication in research design, theory formulation, experimental and computational methods, and scientific communication. The cognitive program at UCLA is designed to ensure exposure to the content and methods deemed essential for all cognitive psychologists, and to provide the opportunity for exposure to additional knowledge and research tools required for specialized professional positions.

The scope of the study of cognition at UCLA is broad, encompassing perception, the control of attention and motor responses, the formation of mental representations, the dynamics of memory retrieval, learning, cognitive development and cognitive breakdown, the mechanisms of reasoning, language, and problem solving. The range of methodological approaches is broad as well. Efforts to formulate computational models of cognitive processes are combined with empirical research and with efforts to specify the neurophysiological underpinnings of cognition. There are applied research opportunities as well in areas such as human-factors engineering, medical technology, education, psychology and the law, and learning/language disabilities.

Modern cognitive psychology is increasingly merging with the broader fields of cognitive science and cognitive neuroscience, which draw together researchers from a variety of disciplines concerned with the study of mind, brain, and intelligence. UCLA has established programs in cognitive science and cognitive neuroscience that sponsor colloquia, seminars, and interdisciplinary research. UCLA is unique in the breadth and depth of its cognitive programs, which draw upon upwards of 40 faculty from such disciplines as cognitive, developmental, and physiological psychology, along with neuroscience, artificial intelligence, linguistics, and philosophy. Each of the major departments involved are among the most highly rated in the country.

The general facilities and resources at UCLA are outstanding. There are few universities with more extensive central libraries, computing facilities, and brain imaging equipment. Overall, the faculty and resources available in the Cognitive Psychology program at UCLA are among the best in the world.

More Cognitive Psychology Information

  • For a list of Required Courses please see the  Psychology Handbook

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.

  • View all journals
  • Explore content
  • About the journal
  • Publish with us
  • Sign up for alerts
  • Journal Club
  • Published: 01 August 2022

COGNITIVE NEUROSCIENCE

The brain basis of cognition

  • Siddharth Ramanan   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0002-8591-042X 1  

Nature Reviews Psychology volume  1 ,  page 491 ( 2022 ) Cite this article

406 Accesses

5 Altmetric

Metrics details

  • Cognitive neuroscience

At the supermarket, you see tomatoes, basil, and mozzarella. Your brain screams “pizza!” Suddenly you are hit with nostalgia and are mentally transported to your recent holiday to the Amalfi coast in Italy. You might wonder how the sensations of these objects transformed into a strong visual of a cherished memory.

For centuries, philosophers and scientists have ardently pursued the links between sensation and cognition. In the past thirty years, scientists have made incredible progress thanks to at least two advances. The first is new technology such as magnetic resonance imaging. By capturing high-definition images of brain structure and function, this technology has revolutionized our ability to visualize how the brain engages with the world. The second is the arrival of common research frameworks, synthesizing previous findings and setting a direction of travel. A key contributor to the latter advance was the foundational review paper by M. Marsel Mesulam.

This is a preview of subscription content, access via your institution

Access options

Subscribe to this journal

Receive 12 digital issues and online access to articles

55,14 € per year

only 4,60 € per issue

Buy this article

  • Purchase on Springer Link
  • Instant access to full article PDF

Prices may be subject to local taxes which are calculated during checkout

Original article

Mesulam, M. From sensation to cognition. Brain 121 , 1013–1052 (1998)

Article   Google Scholar  

Download references

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Medical Research Council Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Siddharth Ramanan

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Siddharth Ramanan .

Ethics declarations

Competing interests.

The author declares no competing interests.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article.

Ramanan, S. The brain basis of cognition. Nat Rev Psychol 1 , 491 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00094-4

Download citation

Published : 01 August 2022

Issue Date : September 2022

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1038/s44159-022-00094-4

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

Quick links

  • Explore articles by subject
  • Guide to authors
  • Editorial policies

Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

research areas in cognitive psychology

  • Virtual Tour
  • Ask the Brain
  • Message from the Director
  • The McGoverns
  • Administration
  • Explore the Brain
  • Polina Anikeeva
  • Emilio Bizzi
  • Martha Constantine-Paton
  • Robert Desimone
  • James DiCarlo
  • Ev Fedorenko
  • Michale Fee
  • Guoping Feng

John Gabrieli

  • Ann Graybiel
  • Mark Harnett
  • H. Robert Horvitz
  • Alan Jasanoff
  • Mehrdad Jazayeri
  • Nancy Kanwisher
  • Josh McDermott
  • Tomaso Poggio
  • Rebecca Saxe
  • Nidhi Seethapathi
  • Guangyu Robert Yang
  • Satrajit Ghosh
  • Dimitrios Pantazis
  • Ian Wickersham
  • Brain Imaging
  • Cellular & Molecular Neuroscience

Cognitive Neuroscience

  • ​Computational Neuroscience
  • Genome Engineering
  • Neurotechnology
  • ​Systems Neuroscience
  • Alzheimer’s Disease
  • ​Autism Spectrum Disorder
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • ​Huntington’s Disease
  • ​Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder
  • ​Parkinson’s Disease
  • ​Schizophrenia
  • Participate in a Study
  • Community Resources
  • Athinoula A. Martinos Imaging Center
  • Poitras Center for Psychiatric Disorders Research
  • Yang Tan Collective
  • Join Our Mailing List
  • Newsletter Archive
  • Sponsored Researchers
  • Meet Our Supporters
  • Events Calendar
  • McGovern Institute Annual Symposium
  • Edward M. Scolnick Prize in Neuroscience
  • Phillip A. Sharp Lecture in Neural Circuits

research areas in cognitive psychology

To view this video please enable JavaScript, and consider upgrading to a web browser that supports HTML5 video

research areas in cognitive psychology

​perception · memory · language · social cognition · attention · empathy · brain disorders · aging · cognitive flexibility · developing brain

Cognitive neuroscientists examine the brain processes that come together to inform our thoughts and understanding of the world.​ Our researchers use a range of approaches, including functional brain imaging and cognitive psychology, to ask fundamental questions about mental processes including how we focus attention, how we process language and music, and how the brain develops and learns.

Gabrieli standing in imaging center

John Gabrieli uses imaging and behavioral tests to understand how the human brain powers learning, thinking, and feeling.

Researchers

The brain puts together a 360˚ panoramic representation of the world around us, even areas behind us using discrete views.

The number of brain regions associated with thinking about the mental states of others ("Theory of Mind").

research areas in cognitive psychology

What is consciousness?

research areas in cognitive psychology

For people who speak many languages, there’s something special about their native tongue

research areas in cognitive psychology

Exposure to different kinds of music influences how the brain interprets rhythm

research areas in cognitive psychology

Research Areas

Cognition research

Faculty members in Cognition investigate a wide range of areas in Cognitive Science using a variety of methods. Topics include language, scene processing, event segmentation, music, statistical learning, memory and consciousness.

Read more about Cognition

Development research

Development

Faculty members in the Development area are focused on interdisciplinary research on lifespan development. They are interested in bringing an interdisciplinary perspective to studying human development in context, as defined by Urie Bronfenbrenner.

Read more about Development

Rat research

Neuroscience

Faculty in the Neuroscience area are interested in the neurophysiological, social/contextual, and evolutionary bases for cognition and behavior in both humans and nonhuman animals.

Read more about Neuroscience

People laughing and socializing

Social and Personality

Faculty members in the Social & Personality area are interested in understanding how people think, feel, and act in real-world social situations.

Read more about Social and Personality

Warning: The NCBI web site requires JavaScript to function. more...

U.S. flag

An official website of the United States government

The .gov means it's official. Federal government websites often end in .gov or .mil. Before sharing sensitive information, make sure you're on a federal government site.

The site is secure. The https:// ensures that you are connecting to the official website and that any information you provide is encrypted and transmitted securely.

  • Publications
  • Account settings
  • Browse Titles

NCBI Bookshelf. A service of the National Library of Medicine, National Institutes of Health.

National Research Council (US) Committee on Military and Intelligence Methodology for Emergent Neruophysiological and Cognitive/Neural Research in the Next Two Decades. Emerging Cognitive Neuroscience and Related Technologies. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2008.

Cover of Emerging Cognitive Neuroscience and Related Technologies

Emerging Cognitive Neuroscience and Related Technologies.

  • Hardcopy Version at National Academies Press

2 Current Cognitive Neuroscience Research and Technology: Selected Areas of Interest

  • INTRODUCTION

Cognitive neuroscience and related technologies constitute a multifaceted discipline that is burgeoning on many fronts. Based on the expertise of its members, and realizing that it could not possibly cover the entire range of science within the discipline, the committee chose to discuss three specific areas of interest: (1) challenges to the detection of psychological states and intentions via neurophysiological activity, (2) neuropsychopharmacology, and (3) functional neuroimaging. Even then, the study’s timeline made it impossible to provide an exhaustive review. Despite these limitations, however, the following discussions accurately depict the current state of cognitive neuroscience research in the selected fields. The chapter also serves as the scientific foundation for Chapter 3 .

  • CHALLENGES TO THE DETECTION OF PSYCHOLOGICAL STATES AND INTENTIONS VIA NEUROPHYSIOLOGICAL ACTIVITY

There is little doubt that great progress has been made over the last quarter century, particularly the last 10 to 15 years, in understanding the physiological and neural bases for psychological processes and behavior. Furthermore, there is a high likelihood that more progress will be made as more sophisticated theoretical models are developed and tested using ever more sophisticated assessment technology. In the applied sector, scientists will probably be better able to identify valid neurophysiological indicators of performance. For example, modeling the human genome will help researchers to index affective, cognitive, and motiva tional states and evaluate the effectiveness of training techniques or to determine the readiness of combat units.

The vast majority of neuroscientific research has been conducted at the group, or aggregate, level rather than at the individual level, and this trend is likely to continue. To achieve sophisticated and highly sensitive neurophysiological assessment of psychological states at the individual level, many significant challenges must be overcome. At a minimum, the neurophysiological indicators will probably have to be individually “tuned” to each user, given the issues of individual variability and plasticity described below.

To accurately assess psychological states using neurophysiological measures, basic neurophysiological work needs to be accomplished over the next two decades. The committee identified and discussed a nonexhaustive list of issues that need to be addressed and questions that need to be answered. These included the nature of psychological states compared to “mind reading,” the nature of neurophysiological and neural activity, and barriers to identification of mental states and intentions.

An important qualification about the parameters necessary for determining psychological state became apparent during the committee’s deliberations—the end use of information about the inferred psychological state. Because technology to infer a psychological state or intention could be put to a broad range of alternative uses, it is important to recognize that acceptable levels of error depend on the differential consequences of a false positive or a missed identification. The technology being applied to determine psychological state could even be derived from an incomplete model of brain function as long as it had sufficient predictive power to accomplish the desired goal. For instance, one would not need a complete model of brain function to construct a brain–computer interface that could improve the self-piloting capabilities of unmanned air vehicles. But the tolerance for error will be much less if a technology is used to determine whether an individual is lying about an act of treason, because the consequences of an error will be greater.

The committee believes that it is critical to fully understand the relationship between neurophysiological markers and actual mental states when the application is the detection of deception.

Mind Reading and Psychological States

It has proven difficult since the beginning of modern psychology 150 years ago to achieve agreement, even among psychologists and other behavioral scientists, on explicit definitions of psychological constructs. Such agreement is important because most psychological constructs bear labels borrowed from common language. Dictionary meanings and usage tempt many scientists to assume that they know the scientific definition of a psychological construct without consulting the scientific literature, where such constructs are explicitly defined.

Typical didactic schemes for organizing psychological constructs imply a more rigid separation between them than actually exists and operates. Today, the main organizing constructs for understanding psychology at the individual level are affect, cognition, and motivation . 1 However, such organization does not necessarily reflect how affective, cognitive, and motivational processes interact. Indeed, attempting to understand each construct in isolation rather than the three as an interdependent triumvirate is to wander in an epiphenomenal domain rather than a realistic psychological domain. If scientists could, for example, accurately determine how a particular soldier processes information about a member of the enemy force (cognition), that knowledge would do very little to help us understand how the soldier will behave toward that enemy unless scientists also take into account how he or she feels about that enemy (affect) and how both constructs play into motivational processes.

When behavioral scientists ask why individuals behave in certain ways, they typically are asking a motivational question. During the first half of the twentieth century, psychologists focused on external environmental factors such as reinforcement to explain motivation. In the latter half of that century, they focused on internal processes to explain affect (moods and emotions) and cognition (information processing, memory) but without knowing details of the causal interconnections among the processes. Today, psychologists understand that behavior occurs between interrelated affective, cognitive, and motivational processes on the one hand and environmental factors and processes on the other. This complex set of interrelated factors must be understood and accounted for to detect a psychological state—that is, to “read” a mind—using any technology.

There has been growing use of the term “mind reading” in the popular press and in a few circumscribed areas of the Department of Defense (DOD). Because the precise meanings of the terms that are used to communicate understanding are critical to the scientific endeavor, the committee believes it is important that the DOD and IC communities understand what is meant in this study by “mind reading” and “psychological state.” Mind reading typically refers to the capacity (imparted by an external mechanism—that is, some form of technology) to determine precisely what an individual is thinking or intending, whether or not the individual is willing to communicate that state of mind. As discussed below, to “read” minds scientists must understand how minds really work to come up with a technology that is of real use, and there are several formidable barriers to achieving such an understanding any time in the next two decades. In contrast, “psychological state” sometimes refers to a broad range of mental activities associated with cognition, affect, and motivation, but more often refers to a discrete and definable mental state, for example, sustained attention (cognition), anger (affect), or hunger (motivation). The committee believes that experimentation, with the careful control of any number of possibly confounding variables, will result in important progress toward understanding the nature of psychological states over the next two decades, using current and yet-to-be developed technologies. It must be understood, however, that much neuroscientific research still infers psychological state based on the experimental controls. Barriers to being able to read minds as well as the hurdles that must be overcome to accurately determine psychological state are discussed below.

The Nature of Neurophysiological Activity

The progress being made by scientific discovery in the field of biology is truly amazing, particularly at the molecular level. At the level of the neural system, however, current knowledge is more speculative. This is understandable given the complexity of the brain. Estimates are that each of the (approximately) 100 billion neurons in the brain synapses—that is, connects—with as many as 50,000 other neurons, making for a large and complicated control network that will likely take decades more of scientific work to map out.

This level of complexity also makes it unnecessary to identify neural centers of activity that are responsible for or associated with specific psychological “modules” of activity. It has been shown that although the neural activity in some brain loci appears to increase or decrease during specified mental activities, these brain loci represent only a small fraction of ongoing neural activity (Raichle, 2007). The rest of the brain is still active, and much more of the operation of the brain system must be understood to develop a firm scientific basis for reliably inferring psychological states.

Barriers to Identifying Psychological States and Intentions via Neural Activity

A science of the relations of mind and brain must show how the elementary ingredients of the former correspond to the elementary functions of the latter. (James, 1890)

The hurdles that must be surmounted in order to detect individual psychological states in a scientifically valid way are quite challenging. Here, several of these challenges are identified and discussed.

Technological Limitations and Advances

The impediment to detection of psychological states via neurophysiological states that is currently the most tractable is availability of technology to monitor and measure putative neurophysiological and neural processes with high spatial and temporal resolution. Although the assessment of peripheral somatic and autonomic systems has been possible for many years (Shapiro and Crider, 1969), advances in the assessment technology have come only recently. Inexpensive, noninvasive endocrine assays (Dickerson and Kemeny, 2004) and noninvasive, high-density electroencephalographic and functional brain imaging technology with high spatial and temporal resolution of brain processes have advanced rapidly. However, scientists must be cautious about what to expect of these technologies in the next quarter century. Technology is yielding new and powerful measurement tools. However, these tools will require sound scientific methods to be of benefit.

Errors in Logic and the Scientific Method

Given that the challenge set forth in the statement of task is to help the intelligence community (IC) and Department of Defense (DOD) “better understand, and therefore forecast, the international neurophysiological and cognitive/neural science research landscape,” members of the committee believe that individuals who are not members of the neuroscientific community tend to make several common errors of logic when they interpret the findings of various technologies that are used to infer psychological states. These errors tend to occur because people misunderstand the relationship between the neurophysiological measurements and the actual mental state that the scientist is attempting to measure. A heightened awareness of the potential for such errors may help the IC and DOD make the best possible decisions when evaluating the scientific claims of researchers in other countries as well as the United States.

Furthermore, because technological innovation is as elemental to certain branches of neuroscientific investigation as the neuroscience itself is, the IC and DOD are likely to encounter two approaches to developing end-user applications of neuroscience, one favored mainly by neural and behavioral scientists, the other by engineers. Both approaches have their strengths, but when evaluating neuroscience, there are important differences. The first approach, as articulated by Cacioppo and Tassinary (1990a), places a premium on plausible scientific theory and the causal relationships underlying the psychological construct and the physiological index. This approach emphasizes the discovery of causal relationships so the theory can be refined and more and more precise hypotheses can be posited, helping to avoid misinterpretation of the data—that is, third-variable confounding, as discussed below. The second approach (the “engineering” one) is to propose, demonstrate, or purport that a given device or technology or method works from a signal detection point of view—for example, “with this technol ogy we can tell when a pilot is too tired to fly with 92.3 percent accuracy.” Any underlying causal model is secondary to the correlated effects. This approach is appealing, works well for many applications, and fits well with the DOD’s proactive approach to problem solving. One significant problem with the largely atheoretical “engineering” approach in neuroscience is that it leaves one open to third-variable confounding, because without a model it is not possible to predict potential confounding. Furthermore, if problems do develop in implementation, there is no model from which to predict the next step. In contrast, the theory-based approach is one of successive approximations by which the underlying theory is continually refined and built upon through the use of models describing the underlying causal relationships.

Relationship Between Neurophysiological Measures and Psychological State

First and foremost, it is important that the reader understand the nature of neuroscientific investigation. When a neuroscientist is studying the biochemistry or the physics involved in brain function—changes in amino acids or the flow of ions, for example—these physiological changes are the phenomenon of interest and the focus of the study. However, when a neuroscientist is studying a psychological state such as attention or anger, changes in brain activity or chemistry are the correlates , or the means by which scientists study the mental state, which is the phenomenon of interest. Whereas physiological changes may regularly accompany a shift in mental state, scientists cannot assume that the mental state bears a one-to-one correspondence with the neural changes they are measuring. A discerning reader might argue, “But what if (and this is a very large if ) scientists knew everything about how the brain functions, and knew how to measure it; would they then, in fact, be measuring mental states?” This line of reasoning, which is often followed by the lay community, is actually a philosophy of science known as reductionism. Reductionism, introduced by Descartes in the seventeenth century, argued that complex things can be fully explained and predicted by reducing them to the interactions of their parts, which are simpler or more fundamental things. He said that the world was a machinelike system that could be understood by taking apart its pieces, studying them, and then putting them back together to see the larger picture. Taken to its logical extreme, measuring the biological mechanisms associated with a mental activity would be equivalent to measuring the mental activity itself rather than just a correlate.

Although a reductionist philosophy of science is accepted in many areas of modern science, including much of physics, chemistry, and microbiology, reductionism to these levels of analysis has never taken hold in the behavioral sciences, probably with good reason. Although reductionists (see, for example, Wilson, 1998) believe that behavior can best be explained by genetic biology and/or the operation of neural control mechanisms, most other scientists argue that reductionist assumptions limit scientific understanding of complex systems. If this is true, mental states may be more than the sum of their parts and may not be amenable to measurement even if the underlying neural activity is fully understood. Stated another way, mental states may emerge only at a psychological level of analysis and cannot be described in terms of purely neurophysiological activity even though the mental states are assumed to be caused by the brain. If reductionism is indeed correct, then at the current level of knowledge about the complexity of neural systems, science is indeed a very long way from being able to read minds from genetic or neural information.

This argument is important because neuroscientists realize that they are measuring the correlates of some mental state, not the mental state itself. As such, the issue of how closely the measures of neural activity map on to the mental state of interest (discussed below) becomes important. This point is of less concern to certain applications of technology to infer brain states (such as augmenting cognition to facilitate the piloting of unmanned aircraft), but it becomes critical when aspects of the psychological state can have legal ramifications, as, for example, in the determination of deception or intent to harm. The knowledge that scientists do not know that the neural activity corresponds one to one with the actual mental state (deceiving) must be weighed very carefully in these instances.

Mapping Measurements of Neurophysiological Activity to Psychological States

The most critical barrier to the identification of a psychological state from its neural signature is the fact that the neural activity underlying the psychological state subserves multiple tasks, so there can therefore be no one-to-one correspondence between neural activity and any psychological state. An excellent example of this point is that of deception detection, or credibility assessment. William Marston, the father of the polygraphy technique for deception detection, believed that there was a unique physiological response during deception. This has proved not to be the case, and few investigators since Marston, including current researchers investigating the use of neural activity measurements to infer deception, believed a unique signature associated with deception would ever be found. Whereas investigators expect to find some consistency in neural response during deception, they do not expect the activated neurons to fire only when the individual is being deceptive and at no other time. Rather, these same neurons are likely to also fire during other types of cognitive and emotional states besides deception (e.g., anxiety, dealing with a heavy cognitive load, inhibiting a pre-potent response). Whereas some low-level physiological processes may have a one-to-one correspondence with neural activation, no higher-order phenomenon on the order of a mental state has been found to have this type of neural pattern. Accordingly, researchers investigating the neural correlates of psychological states must control for many other variables, including other mental states, that could account for the neural activity they are measuring to be more certain that their results are indeed due to the construct under investigation (say, anxiety rather than deception or vice versa).

Fortunately, there is a very useful approach to proper inference between indexes and psychological constructs or states originally suggested by Cacioppo and Tassinary (1990b), who elucidated four types of neurophysiological index for psychological constructs: outcomes, concomitants, markers, and invariants. Awareness of this typology helps us to recognize important inferential problems associated with putative neurophysiological and neural indices of psychological states. Whereas the goal of a neurophysiological index for a psychological construct may be a symmetric, one-to-one relationship between the index and the variable based on a plausible and verifiable scientific theory, in practice this is rare. 2 To be symmetric, the presence of the variable must always be accompanied by the presence of the index and vice versa, and the two must covary systematically. To be based on a plausible scientific theory, the underlying causal relationships between the psychological construct and the physiological index should be valid ones.

More commonly, neurophysiological indices are outcomes and concomitants. Outcomes and concomitants are merely associations or correlations between a physiological response (or set of responses) and a psychological construct that are context bound or context free, respectively (see Figure 2-1 ).

Associations between a physiological response (or set of responses) and a psychological construct. SOURCE: Cacioppo and Tassinary (1990b) ©1990 by the American Psychological Association. Adapted with permission. The use of APA information does (more...)

Neither enjoys a symmetric one-to-one relationship between the response and the construct. For instance, the sympathetically driven autonomic responses indicative of stress is an outcome within the Cacioppo and Tassinary framework (1990b)—that is, it is context dependent and asymmetric. In a different context (the diagnostic one Erasistratus found himself in), such responses could be related to different psychological states (e.g., love or anxiety).

Markers and invariants are associations between a physiological response and a psychological construct that are context-bound or context-free, respectively, but do enjoy a symmetric one-to-one relationship ( Figure 2-1 ). There are few (see below) well-validated symmetric peripheral or central nervous system (CNS) neurophysiological markers of affective, cognitive, and motivational psychological constructs. This paucity is partially due to poor/insufficient understanding of how neurophysiological systems operate and the resulting lack of sophisticated and validated biopsychosocial theory, which have facilitated the development of valid markers and invariants of psychological states.

Symmetric (one-to-one correspondence) relationships have rarely, if ever, been shown to exist between psychological constructs and their neurophysiological indicators. This lack of a symmetric relationship is a major problem for detecting psychological states from the indicators. Neurophysiological and neural activities are almost always multifunctional when it comes to causing underlying psychological constructs. So, even if every time an individual enters a psychological state (e.g., “love”) the same portion of cortex (i.e., the left prefrontal cortex) is activated, does not mean that every time that portion of the cortex is activated the person is in that psychological state—that is to say, neurological measures may be sensitive, but are rarely specific. Good science avoids this logical error known as the “affirmation of the consequent.”

These errors of inference can be avoided by precisely specifying the circumstances or “controls” under which the data can be interpreted by limiting the number psychological states. For instance, a brain–computer interface designed to assess attention to an external task and that has been accompanied by individual training for the user may place sufficient limits on both the environment and the user’s possible states to allow accurate and useful interpretation of the neural responses. This highly controlled scenario, however, which has controls similar to the experimental controls that are used to interpret neuroimaging data does not amount to mind reading or to determining intent from the raw neural signals. Rather, a cognitive state is inferred based on the controls placed on the situation and is still subject to potential signal detection errors (e.g., false positives, false negatives, misses).

Avoiding Errors of Inference

Fortunately, a reductionist philosophy of science is not a requisite for drawing valid inferences about psychological states from neurophysiological and neural activity if one accepts the “identity thesis” as a basic metaphysical assumption. This assumption states that all psychological phenomena occur via bodily processes and is widely shared by behavioral scientists and neuroscientists (Cacioppo and Tassinary, 1990a; Blascovich, 2000; Blascovich and Seery, 2007). Accordingly, there is nothing ethereal about human behavior, and all psychological states are embodied somehow. If one can associate certain neurophysiological data with certain psychological states, then identifying psychological states from such information is a potentially tractable, though very difficult, challenge. Several logical and inferential issues, including those associated with the section below on the third variable problem, cause this challenge to be daunting.

The Third Variable Problem. When two variables, such as a psychological state and some specified neurophysiological measure, are related probabilistically, even if perfectly so, scientists cannot assume that they are causally related. For example, the correlation between shoe size and reading ability in children might be a spurious correlation. There is no doubt that both increase with age; however, correlation does not imply causality. Correlation is necessary for causality, but two other criteria must be met to imply causality: (1) time ordering (the cause must occur before the effect) and (2) third variables must be ruled out. Whereas an engineering approach can be used to determine time sequencing, a scientific model could allow ruling out third variables as the cause of a correlation; however, a poor or incomplete model will allow for many interpretations of an effect that might have an altogether different cause. This becomes a significant problem, for instance, when one wishes to decide whether a person is lying on a polygraph test; even if there is a high correlation between guilt and strong autonomic reactions to certain questions, it would be a mistake to conclude that guilt is causing the stronger reactions if anxiety, not guilt, can produce those same reactions.

The goal of a neurophysiological index of a psychological construct is a symmetric, one-to-one relationship between the index and the variable based on a plausible and verifiable scientific theory. To be symmetric, the variable must always be accompanied by the index and vice versa, and the two must covary systematically. To be based on a plausible scientific theory, the underlying causal relationships between the psychological construct and the physiological index should be valid.

Brain Plasticity

Brain plasticity refers to changes that occur in brain organization and function as a result of experience. There is now considerable evidence that brain activity associated with a psychological state or process can change throughout life as a function of factors such as sleep, maturation, experience, damage, exogenous (e.g., pharmacological) agents or a combination of these. Indeed, most poststroke rehabilitation therapy (e.g., relearning walking, talking) would be ineffective if such change were not possible.

Brain plasticity is manifested in at least three ways. One involves functional shifts and changes that occur when control of motoric behavior reorganizes itself in a different area of the cortex as a result of experience. A second way, termed synaptic plasticity, involves changes in neuroreceptor production and/or sensitivity that potentiate or antagonize the likelihood of synaptic transmission. A third way, at least speculatively, brain plasticity may manifest itself is changes in brain structure; that is, actual changes in the number of neurons and synapses, the most obvious examples of which are increases occurring early in life and decreases occurring as a result of lesions or aging.

Brain plasticity represents a challenge to those seeking to develop neuronal indexes for psychological states—i.e., outcomes, concomitants, markers, and invariants—on an individual level, because structural, organizational, and functional differences between individuals—and within them over time—will have to be accounted for. It is also possible that a high degree of plasticity-based error in any given index could reduce its sensitivity and specificity and, hence, its practical value for “reading” individual minds. However, this remains an open question, for scientists do not yet know how plasticity might affect any given set of measures across various populations.

Variability Within and Between Individuals

Two important challenges to using brain states to index psychological states are variability between individuals and also within a single individual. It seems likely that brain plasticity, along with genomic factors, may be one of the underlying causes of such variability, which apparently exists. However, “it is not easy to change the habits of people who are comfortable with traditional ways of doing things, and developers of cognitive models have continued to rely for support mainly on the fitting of functions such as curves of learning, retention, and generalization to averaged data” (Estes, 2002).

Estes has examined the relationships between typical brain scan images aggregated across individuals and those of the individual cases from which aggregated images are derived. Figure 2-2 illustrates the problem of individual variability for location of episodic memory in the brain. The leftmost image is the group or aggregate image. The next three images illustrate some of the individual cases from which the aggregate image was derived. None of the individual images match the group image. Hence, it would be inappropriate to base a neural index of the operation of episodic memory on the aggregate picture without adjustment for individual differences.

Individual variability in the case of the putative location of episodic memory in the brain. SOURCE: Reprinted with permission from unpublished work (Miller, 2007).

Miller concludes that “investigations into the sources of unique individual brain activity may be necessary in order to understand the dynamic patterns of brain activity that underlie widely distributed, strategy-filled tasks like episodic memory” (Miller, 2007). There appears, moreover, to be variability across tasks, with some tasks showing more or less variability of locus than others. Furthermore, Miller reported that within-subject location appeared to be relatively stable across time, such that the same areas were activated in the same individual by the same task performed 6 months apart (Miller et al., 2002a,b). Individualized approaches to functional mapping, however, are feasible under certain circumstances. For instance, if a government wanted to outfit trained combat pilots flying high-tech aircraft with some type of neural interface, it would make sense to invest in determining individualized neurophysiological markers. Accounting for individual variability is both reasonable and feasible, because within-subject variability across time (after accounting for reorganization due to learning) tends to be low for cognitive tasks (Miller, 2007). This cost-benefit argument is more of a problem when attempting to apply a generic (averaged) algorithm to a given individual without individual tuning.

Specificity of Psychological States Within Contexts

The likelihood of developing valid neurophysiological indexes for inferring psychological states or intentions depends on the required specificity of the state itself and the specificity of the context within which it occurs. It is typically easier to develop an accurate index of a given psychological state if the number of candidate states can be limited by experimental controls. Consider, as an example, a vigilance scenario in the cognitive domain. It would be simpler to develop a neurophysiological index that marks a person as consciously processing information at a general level (e.g., having or not having the resources to cope with the situation he or she is in) than at a more specific level (e.g., having or not having specific resources). In the affective domain, it would be simpler to develop an index that distinguishes the polarity of a superordinate affective state (e.g., negative versus positive) than an index that distinguishes between more specific superordinate states (e.g., anger or fear). In the motivational domain, it would be easier to develop an index that distinguishes between fairly general superordinate motivational states (e.g., avoidance or approach) than to develop one that distinguishes more specific states (e.g., take flight or surrender).

Simultaneously indexing superordinate categories of general cognitive, affective, and motivational psychological states based on neurophysiological information can have great practical value because these general categories of psychological states are highly interdependent and simultaneous. Hence, if one could index all three categories simultaneously in a given context (say, warfighting), the resulting combined index would be more useful for drawing inferences and making predictions than any single index. Furthermore, one would be able to make these predictions without asking the warfighter questions and one could refine the predictions to reflect these continuously available indexes.

Indeed, one can probably infer psychological or behavioral intentions more accurately based on the combination index. If the cognitive index revealed that a soldier evaluates his or her resources as sufficient to meet the perceived needs in the particular context (e.g., a battle), if the affective index revealed that the soldier is experiencing positive affect (e.g., challenge) rather than negative affect (e.g., threat), and if the motivational index revealed that the soldier is motivated to approach rather than avoid the situation, one would could reasonably conclude that the soldier was prepared or even intended to do battle. On the other hand, if one index revealed that the soldier cognitively evaluates his or her resources as insufficient, another revealed that he or she is experiencing negative affect, and a third revealed that the soldier is avoidance-oriented, one could reasonably conclude that the individual was ready to retreat.

Existing peripheral neurophysiological measures include the following: (1) hemodynamic markers of cognitive resource/demand evaluations—for example, reciprocal changes in cardiac output and total peripheral resistance (Blascovich and Tomaka, 1996), (2) electromyographic indexes of affect—for example, relative reciprocal increases and decreases in zygomaticus major and corrugator supercillii muscle movements in the face (Cacioppo et al., 1986), and (3) cardiac indexes of approach/avoidance motivation—for example, increases or decreases in cardiac ventricular contractility and heart rate (Blascovich and Tomaka, 1996). If one could develop CNS indexes of superordinate categories of cognitive, affective, and motivational state, there would be a useful redundancy.

Assumptions About Base Rates of Psychological States and Intentions

Just as they like to rely on averaged data (Estes identified this propensity in 2002), many scientists also assume that base rates of dichotomous psychological states are 50/50. Much as scientists know that not everyone or every brain is average, they also realize that the probability that a person will act in one way rather than another is not necessarily 50 percent. This realization notwithstanding, development and even validational work on indexes of psychological states often assumes a base rate of 50 percent. 3

If base rates of psychological states, such as intentions to commit an act of terrorism or to defect, were 50 percent, individuals with such intentions would be much easier to identify. Unfortunately, many important psychological states are infrequent events, with the consequence that indexes validated assuming a 50 percent base rate produce a very large number of mistaken detections. Green and Swets’s classic paper on signal detection theory (1966) detailed the logic and mathematics for inferring the existence of target states from signals (or indexes) as a function of base rates.

Mistaken detections can be avoided only when a target signal is in almost perfectly symmetric (i.e., 1 to 1) correspondence with the target event in the biological or other system. Additionally, systems for transmitting and identifying the target signal must be almost perfectly accurate (Imrey, 2007). Imrey further argues that (1) combinations of these two circumstances are rare; (2) intuition on this point is notoriously poor, partly because of the ways accuracy is commonly expressed; (3) known biases in data collection invariably lead to overoptimism; (4) overoptimism means overconfidence in the reliability of results, and (5) this means underestimation of false positives and false negatives. A specific discussion on base rates and the detection of deception is found later in the chapter under the case study of detecting deception.

Finding 2-1. Cognitive neuroscientists can identify neurophysiological markers of general psychological states—for example, positive versus negative affect, automatic versus controlled cognitive processing, approach versus avoidance motivation, attention versus inattention—within individuals in specified contexts but are not yet able to identify highly specific psychological states and intentions—that is, exactly what a particular person is thinking, intending, or doing. Given the current state of knowledge about brain neurophysiology, it is highly likely that any advances in collective knowledge about individual psychological states over the next two decades will continue to occur in highly controlled situations where the number of candidate mental states is limited. The ability to determine a person’s mental state strictly from neurophysiological markers without environmental controls is unlikely to be gained any time in the next two decades.

Detection of Deception as an Example of Efforts to Identify Accurate Neurophysiological Indexes of Specific Psychological States in Individuals

The concept of the detection of deception was recently broadened and is now known as “credibility assessment,” and includes additional factors such as source verification and witness corroboration. The concept is of considerable interest to the IC and DOD. Detection of deception can serve to illustrate the considerations discussed above as barriers to the identification of accurate neurophysiological indices of specific psychological states in individuals (Feinberg and Stern, 2005).

At the societal level, the capacity to detect deception is valued for many social, legal, and medical reasons. At the government level, the capacity to detect deception is valued because those having or seeking authority and/or power need to be able to safeguard the national interest. At the individual level, detection of deception helps to identify individuals who threaten some important value of a social group. In addition, it often makes sense to help determine the reliability of information or witnesses before resources are allocated. As such, technologies for the detection of deception entail identifying a specific kind of psychological state at the individual, or idiographic, level.

Not unlike in the scientific community at large, there was considerable debate among members of the committee about the ability to develop useful neurophysiological markers of deception. The committee believed, however, that it is critical for the IC and DOD to understand where opinions differed. Box 2-1 summarizes the key points of agreement and disagreement on detection of deception.

Committee Agreements and Disagreements on Detection of Deception. Agreement The committee agreed that, as outlined in previous NRC reports, traditional measures of deception detection technology have proven to be insufficiently accurate. Second, the committee (more...)

Previous Research on Detection of Deception

No known lie-detection technology has been sufficiently accurate (i.e., virtually inerrant) to be legally acceptable, especially given the relatively low base rates for many types of threats to a social order. As described above, the main technology used during the last 100 years in the United States and a few other countries (e.g., Israel, Canada, and Japan) is the polygraph, the generic name for an instrument that can record multiple physiological measurements which has come to be closely associated with the lie detection technique utilizing it. Conventional polygraphy relies on psychophysiological measures of the sympathetic nervous system response (respiration rate, heart rate, electrodermal activity) to detect anxiety associated with guilt or lying (OTA, 1990). As determinants of psychological states, however, these autonomic response suffer, in particular, from a lack of specificity and cannot differentiate guilt from other cognitive/ affective states—say anxiety—resulting in an unacceptably high level of false positives (Office of Technology Assessment, 1983; Iacono, 2000). A report on the polygraph and lie detection (NRC, 2003, p. 4) concluded that:

… in populations of examinees such as those represented in the polygraph research literature, untrained in countermeasures, specific-incident [i.e., criminal] polygraph tests can discriminate lying from truth telling at rates well above chance but below perfection.

However, this should not be understood to be the applied accuracy of polygraph testing in operational field situations. Readers should note that many of the examinees referred to in the quote above are research subjects who, for the most part, are often informed of the nature of the research beforehand and are not typically drawn from the population to whom polygraph tests are typically given (individuals being investigated for criminal activity, those seeking employment in sensitive national security positions, prisoners of war). Often participants are college students or other volunteers and are hardly representative of populations that are likely to threaten the social order. Hence, the NRC report goes on to conclude that “polygraph testing yields an unacceptable choice for … employee security screening between too many … employees falsely judged as deceptive and too many major security threats left undetected” (NRC, 2003, p. 6).

It is the lack of specificity, in particular, of these autonomic measures to discriminate deception from other affective states that leads to insufficient positive and negative predictive power. That is, autonomic responses, at least as measured by the polygraph, are “outcomes” only, could represent a number of psychological states, and must be interpreted within a highly specified context. It is not surprising, then, that both the IC and the DOD have been interested in more precise (more sensitive and specific) measures of deception detection. As for other deception detection techniques, the report concluded that:

Some potential alternatives to the polygraph show promise, but none has yet been shown to outperform even the polygraph. None shows any promise of supplanting the polygraph for screening purposes in the near term. (NRC, 2003, pp. 7-8)

However, the report (NRC, 2003, p. 174) goes on to state that:

Functional brain imaging techniques have important advantages over the polygraph, in theory, because they examine directly what the brain is doing…. Not enough is yet known about the specific cognitive or emotional processes that accompany deception, about their localization in the brain, or about whether imaging signals can differentiate the brain activity associated with these processes from brain activity associated with other processes to make an assessment of the potential validity of these techniques on the grounds of the basic science. Further research with fMRI, coupled with a scientifically based cognitive psychological approach to deception, will be needed to determine if these issues can be addressed…. If a research effort is undertaken to find improved scientific techniques for the detection of deception, basic research on brain imaging would be a top candidate for the research agenda.

For illustrative purposes, the committee next discusses deception detection work in light of the challenges described above. As noted, there was considerable debate among the committee members about what would constitute a practical or useful application of technology to the assessment of psychological states.

Deception and the Nature of Neurophysiological Measures

Understanding the nature of the relationship between physiological markers and the psychological states that are purportedly assessed by quantifying the markers is critical for several reasons. First, users of the technology must recognize the limitations of the tools they are using. The majority of polygraphers do not make the mistake of believing that they are measuring deception directly via neurophysiological information, avoiding an unwarranted reductionistic approach. Rather, they claim that certain neurophysiological changes (e.g., respiration, heart rate, blood pressure, electrodermal activity), as a set, pattern themselves differently during acts of deception than during acts of truth telling. This places a burden on the methods used to gather the psychophysiological or neurological data, as proper interpretation of the data fundamentally relies on appropriate methodological technique and controls.

While it can be tempting to assume that brain imaging or other neurophysiological measurements allow direct access to the psychological state under investigation, this assumption is not warranted. Based on research to date, it is highly improbable that there is any specific “lie circuit” in the brain that is dedicated to deception. Rather, the neural circuits that respond during deception will respond in other, nondeceptive circumstances, and only appropriate techniques will allow accurate interpretation of the data. The committee agrees that important legal decisions should not be made as if incontrovertible proof of deception existed if they are based only on the correlates of deception.

Unfortunately, the biopsychosocial theory underlying the putative relationships between the autonomic measures that are obtained with a traditional polygraph and their actual value as accurate indices of lying remains unsophisticated and essentially unchanged for nearly a century. Twenty-five years ago (1983), the United States Office of Technology Assessment issued a report prepared by a group of scientists who had evaluated the scientific validity of polygraph testing for lie detection (OTA, 1983). The report concluded that “the basic theory of polygraph testing is only partially developed and researched…. A stronger theoretical base is needed for the entire range of polygraph applications” (NRC, 2003, p. 6).

This statement largely reflects the paucity of well-designed and controlled studies for investigating the potential confounds when a strong inference is drawn from polygraphy data collected under a great variety of conditions. From a neuroscientific perspective, theory must be refined through experimentation. The report leveled the same criticism at the theoretical basis of polygraphic lie detection (NRC, 2003). Indeed, a long line of similar reports, including one in the archives of the National Research Council as early as 1917 and another in 1954 (Guertin and Wilhelm, 1954), made the same point.

This may be due to the fact that there are essentially two communities when it comes to judging the utility of the polygraph in determining deception, the polygraph community and the scientific community (Porges, 2006). These two communities base their judgments on different criteria. A critical difference between them has been their approach to research: The polygraph community has long taken an applied, problem-oriented approach, with efficacy as its goal, driven by perceived societal needs. The scientific community, in contrast, takes a basic, theory-driven approach that emphasizes understanding the mechanisms underlying the process of deception, driven by the principles of science as they relate to society. These two approaches are often conceptualized as inherently divergent, which they certainly have been in practice. However, lacking a scientific basis, the applied approach has failed to increase its efficacy or advance its credibility over several decades, as outlined above.

The literature on lie-detection research, which has long focused mostly on measurements of autonomic reactions, appears to value seemingly high correlations between particular neurophysiological responses and the prevarication they identify, independent of sound biopsychosocial theory. Continued reliance on a century-old, outdated physiology-based rationale has resulted in a failure to advance the underlying science. The best that can be said for the current physiological indexes of lying used in polygraph testing is that they are outcomes, the type of index that provides the logically weakest basis for inference (Cacioppo and Tassinary, 1990b). Put even more succintly, the lack of biopsychosocial theoretical sophistication underlying polygraphic lie detection techniques over the past century has led to entropy in the field of lie detection. Because no known physiological or neurophysiological measure enjoys a one-to-one relationship with deception, all correlates of deception have multiple causes, only one of which is deception. This being the case, protocols and uses that rely on very few or even a single response to determine deception will be highly susceptible to false positives, despite which such protocols continue to be used (see, for example, Tsiamyrtzis et al., 2007). This has been demonstrated many times from work in such fields as event-related potentials, where multiple trials have proved much more reliable than single trials. If a marker is a true correlate of the deceptive state, it should survive several response trials, capitalizing on Bayesian probability. (Although it is well known that autonomic responses habituate, they can serve as markers for a few trials.)

The committee agrees that an integration of basic, theory-driven science with an applied, problem-oriented approach could facilitate acceptable solutions to the assessment of credibility. According to Porges (2006), basic science can help delineate the neural processes underlying deception and the theoretical relationships among the psychological state(s), their neural and physiological markers, their measurement, and their application in the field.

Improvements in Technology

Given that standard polygraph technology is no different in basic measurement principles than it was a century ago and, according to the literature, has failed to produce sufficient reliability, it is not surprising that many lie-detection researchers have turned to newer CNS assessment technologies, including high-density electroencephalography, near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to improve the accruracy with which it detects lies. A small formative body of published research on the neural circuitry associated with deception utilizes various neuroimaging techniques. Recent studies using positron emission tomography (PET) and fMRI have provided insights into the neural circuitry associated with deception, with specific areas in the prefrontal cortices and amygdala being the most commonly implicated regions (Abe et al., 2006, 2007; Mohamed et al., 2006; Davatzikos et al., 2005; Langleben et al., 2002, 2005; Lee et al., 2002, 2005; Nuñez et al., 2005; Phan et al., 2005a; Kozel et al., 2004a,b, 2005; Ganis et al., 2003; Spence et al., 2001). Recent fNIRS studies of deception have also implicated prefrontal brain regions in the neural circuitry associated with deception (Bunce et al., 2005). Consistent with the Bunce et al. (2005) observation, through the end of 2007, all published neuroimaging studies of deception except one (Langleben et al., 2002), including PET, fNIRS and fMRI technologies, studies of well-practiced vs. spontaneous lies (Ganis et al., 2003), studies of malingering, and cultural samples (Lee et al., 2005, with Chinese subjects, Bunce et al., 2005, with East Indian subjects) have found activation in a similar area of the dorsolateral/ventrolateral prefrontal cortex. Another recently published study correlated fMRI images with standard skin conductance measurements during a concealed information paradigm, with interesting results (Gamer et al., 2007). Although some members of the committee believed this preliminary body of work to be quite promising there has not yet been sufficient systematic research to determine if functional neuroimaging can meet the challenges to the neurophysiological detection of psychological states relevant to deception, as described above. Future research is warranted in the brain plasticity and variability; specificity of psychological states; and base rates.

Brain Plasticity and Individual Variability

An important divergence between traditional polygraphic lie detection and newer lie detection based on newer CNS assessment technologies is the latter’s susceptibility to problems created by the brain’s plasticity and resultant individual variability in CNS structure and function. For example, if the loci of CNS synaptic transmissions underlying prevarication vary across individuals or within an individual across time, those loci would not be accurate enough to be useful as neurophysiological indexes of lying for individual testing. This is still an open question because to date, no sufficient research on these issues has been published. If individual variability proves to be a significant factor, there will be no scientific basis for using the newer assessment technologies unless theoretical mechanisms relating such variability to the biopsychsocial basis for lying can be specified and validated. This does not mean that a theory could not be developed, and some preliminary models have already been proposed (Porges, 2006; Spence, 2004). However, a great deal of appropriate research needs to be done before a specific conclusion could be drawn about the validity of CNS measures of intentional deception, with an interactive process between the scientific model and the research. While this does not rule out any role for an applied, problem-oriented approach—for example, does the outcome or marker predict at a given level of sensitivity/specificity—a good model would help to avoid serious misinterpretation of the data.

In addition to CNS-based measurement, several other sensing techniques are being investigated for their potential ability to discern deception or concealed knowledge. These techniques may avoid some of the issues surrounding neural plasticity, but they must still meet appropriate criteria for accuracy. Some are remote, noncontact sensing techniques that measure autonomic function and are already in use. For instance, laser Doppler vibrometry is a remote sensing technique for heart rate, blood pressure, and several other physical properties. Although the technique does provide more information about heart function than a polygraph, it typically measures aspects of autonomic function associated with stress in response to threat. Other similar techniques include voice stress analysis, pupilometry, and infrared measures of the periorbital regions.

One measurement that appears to have a largely cognition-based etiology is that of eye-movement-based memory assessment (EMMA) (see, for example Marchak, 2006). This method is based on evidence that people scan faces they have previously seen in a different way than they scan novel faces (Altoff and Cohen, 1999). People reliably use fewer eye fixations, sample fewer regions, and fewer statistical constraints in viewing familiar rather than new faces. By tracking and quantifying these eye movement patterns using the appropriate technology and experimental controls, researchers can identify concealed knowledge. These patterns can also be combined with performance measures such as speed and accuracy, which show increased efficiency in the subject’s processing of previously learned materials. The technique has been applied to objects and scene recognition as well as faces. EMMA, which stems from a theory-driven model of memory and adaptive function, appears to be promising. Published levels of correct classification across studies (grand mean = 88.1 percent) are on a par with results from standard polygraphs. More work is needed on the level of positive and negative predictive power across various samples of individuals. In addition, the methodology itself is somewhat constraining because it requires a specific type of knowledge on the part of the subject and a specific set of stimuli.

Specificity of Psychological States

The specificity of a psychological state should be reflected in the specificity of the neurophysiological index created for it. The neurophysiological index for a more general or superordinate psychological state is likely to be more easily validated than the corresponding index for a more specific psychological state. Deception in everyday matters is not as general a psychological state as information processing or fear or anxiety, but it is not as specific as lying about something as portentous as, say terrorism. Developers of new technologies must avoid making the same type of error. Turning to measurement of CNS response to deception (may represent) an attempt to develop an appropriate level of analysis for deception.

Reconsideration of Base Rates

The challenge posed by base rates can be overcome by applying signal detection theory, as mentioned above. Here is a substantive example. Assume that deception has a base rate of 1 in 1,000, and an index that is 90 percent accurate and that 10,000 individuals are being screened using the index. This assumed base rate is probably high if scientists are attempting to identify spies or terrorists in organizations as large as the military or in the populations of individuals on a given day traveling by air to or within the United States. As Table 2-1 from a report on the polygraph and lie detection illustrates (NRC, 2003), 1,598 people, or 99.5 percent of those who failed the screen and were (incorrectly) identified as spies or terrorists, would be false positives and 2 people (20 percent of the actual spies) who passed the screen and were identified as nonspies or nonterrorists would be false negatives. Only 8 (0.5 percent) of the people who failed the screen would actually be spies or terrorists and therefore true positives.

TABLE 2-1. Rates of False Positives and False Negatives from Polygraph Examinations.

Rates of False Positives and False Negatives from Polygraph Examinations.

If the detection threshold is lowered to reduce the number of false positives, as Table 2-2 from the NRC report illustrates, the number of spies or terrorists who passed the screen (false negatives) would increase. In this case, 8 people (80 percent) of the spies or terrorists would have passed the screen. This example emphasizes the importance of a highly accurate test when testing large numbers of people for low-base-rate events, and calls attention to the care that must be taken in using the information when a less accurate test is involved.

TABLE 2-2. Increase in the Number of Spies or Terrorists Who Passed the Screen (False Negatives).

Increase in the Number of Spies or Terrorists Who Passed the Screen (False Negatives).

These statistics make evident an important conclusion. When looking for targets in settings with a low base rate, such as screenings for terrorists at airports, achieving high confidence in a positive detection requires using a technology with an extremely high discriminating capacity on the order of that associated with HIV testing or the even more accurate DNA testing. If such a level of accuracy cannot be achieved using the test technology alone, further information that additionally differentiates true targets, and is external to the test technology, needs to be obtained and brought to bear.

Finding 2-2. The committee recognizes the IC’s strong interest in improving its ability to detect deception. Consistent with the 2003 NRC study The Polygraph and Lie Detection , the committee uniformly agreed that, to date, insufficient, high-quality research has been conducted to provide empirical support for the use of any single neurophysiological technology, including functional neuroimaging, to detect deception.

Opinions differed within the committee concerning the near-term contribution of functional neuroimaging to the development of a system to detect deception in a practical or forensic sense. Committee members who conduct neuroimaging research largely agreed that studies published to date are promising and that further research is needed on the potential for neuroimaging to provide a more accurate method to determine deception. Importantly, human institutional review board standards require, at a minimum, that individuals not be put at any greater risk than they would be in their normal everyday lives. The committee believes that certain situations would allow such testing under “normal risk” situations; although the committee strongly endorses the necessity of realistic, but ethical, research in this area, it does not specify the nature of that research in this report.

Recommendation 2-1. The committee recommends further research on multimodal methodological approaches for detecting and measuring neurophysiological indicators of psychological states and intentions. This research should combine multiple measures and assessment technologies, such as imaging techniques and the recording of electrophysiological, biochemical, and pharmacological responses. Resources invested in further cognitive neuroscience research should support programs of research based on scientific principles and that avoid the inferential biases inherent in previous research in polygraphy.

  • NEUROPSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY

Drugs and other mind-altering chemicals can influence all aspects of human psychology, including cognition, emotion, motivation, and performance. For known drugs, predictions of the type, onset, magnitude, and/or duration of effects in individuals or groups can be limited by incomplete knowledge of the interacting processes that govern drug effects. Clinical, field, and research experience reveals that drug effects in individual humans arise from interactions of multiple factors, including (but not limited to) the drug itself; its dose and route of use; the demand characteristics of the current situation; and the individual’s health, physiology, and experience with drugs and performance demands. Attempts to predict the effects of new drugs are hampered by the possible surprise in structure, targets, delivery, mechanisms of action, interactions with other drugs, and varying performance conditions.

In the following discussion the committee uses the term “drug” to refer to any chemical agent with the capacity to alter human affect, cognition, motivation, or performance. Agents that can do this include not only pharmaceutical drugs for preventing, treating, or mitigating disease symptoms, but also foods, hormones, intoxicants, nutrients, plants, poisons, supplements, toxins, and so on. The porous boundaries among these classifications is exemplified by botulinum toxin type A, available by prescription as Botox. The actions and effects of drugs may change with long-term use or under altered conditions; may interact with medical, occupational, physical and psychological conditions; may affect individuals differently; and may affect human functions other than psychology. Effects may be perceived as beneficial, harmful, or neutral, and such perceptions may change with conditions.

Most discussions of drugs and their effects are organized along the lines of current models of brain and nervous system functioning. This method of organization can help to identify likely effects of known drugs but probably should not be used to identify potential dual-use threats. Changes in models of brain function may create new and surprising ideas about how, when, where, or why drugs produce their effects; about what those effects are; about the kinds of chemicals that function as drugs to alter human functioning; and about ways to enhance, minimize, or counteract drug effects. It is particularly important to realize that the drugs that changed psychiatry in the mid-twentieth century were not predicted by many pharmacological or psychological models of their time, perhaps especially in the United States (Swazey, 1974). Rather, the brief history of neuropsychopharmacology illustrates how the expectations of a particular cultural, medical, and research climate may cause a failure to predict new drugs, new ways of using drugs, or new drug effects. Recent advances in neuropsychopharmacology that have the potential to be “game changers” include a much improved knowledge of brain function and delivery systems such as are enabled by nanotechnology that would allow substances to cross the blood-brain barrier.

Many current psychotherapeutic drugs—that is, drugs used in treatment and management of psychiatric disorders—and their likely mechanisms of action were not anticipated by prior research or theory (Barrett, 2007). Classic examples of psychotherapeutic drugs with unexpected mechanisms of action and/or unanticipated effects, and called to attention by alert clinicians, include lithium, chlorpromazine, monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitors, and tricyclic antidepressants (Cade, 1949; Jarvik, 1970; Swazey, 1974). Another unexpected discovery was lead poisoning, which had long-lasting psychological effects in the employees of a workshop where batteries were made (Hamilton, 1915; International Labour Office, 1934). These examples suggest that new drugs with marked effects on critical psychological functions are “black swans,” unanticipated events with effects that could not have been predicted, that may fail to be observed when they happen, or that have large, long-lasting consequences that go unrecognized (Taleb, 2007a,b). 4

One modern drug with psychological effects that were not predicted by initial descriptions of its clinical pharmacology is ketamine. Developed in the private sector as an anesthetic, then discovered in clinical use to have hallucinogenic effects and co-opted into the club drug scene as Special K, it is now under investigation in the public and private sectors as a rapidly acting antidepressant and for the treatment of chronic pain; the use of ketamine as a rapid acting antidepressant remains unreplicated and provocative. Other such drugs are opioid antagonists as a treatment for alcoholism and cannabinoids. These examples suggest that current models of neuropsychopharmacological effect account post hoc for psychological effects of drugs and may have poor predictive ability.

In spite of the difficulty of predicting their psychological effects, psychotherapeutic drugs have proven to have greater clinical effectiveness than many other treatments. This has had important consequences for medical and cultural expectations about drugs. First, the discovery and clinical use of drugs for treatment and management of psychiatric disease have had far-reaching effects on research and practice in psychiatry, psychology, cognitive science, and other mental health and behavioral sciences. Demonstration that particular drugs could effectively treat psychiatric illnesses suggested that such illnesses were treatable much like other illnesses and promoted the medicalization of psychiatry. Equally important, the clinical usefulness of the earliest such drugs provided insight into the brain mechanisms that mediate the drugs’ therapeutic efficacy and created opportunities for research to understand the chemical and biological bases of their effects. The critical linkage between drug effects and discoveries of brain receptors for neurotransmitters and other endogenous brain chemicals enabled the development of drugs with fewer side effects and more focused activity as well as a few truly new drugs that could target newly discovered chemical systems in the brain.

The appearance of clinically useful brain drugs occurred as the field of neuroscience coalesced so that medical and behavioral researchers could explore how biology manifests itself in behavior (Barrett, 2007; Dinges, 2007). One marker of the importance of the emerging field of neuroscience is the growth in attendance at the annual meeting of the premier professional association in the field, the Society for Neuroscience, from 1,396 in 1971 to 34,815 in 2005 (Society for Neuroscience, 2007). 5 Discoveries in neuroscience can be exploited to create new cellular or subcellular targets for drugs, new drug delivery systems, and new strategies to direct or control drug effects and achieve desired psychological effects. Novel classes of drugs approved by the Food and Drug Administration (FDA) illustrate the potential surprises of research, including drugs for erectile dysfunction, which arose from chemicals that were targeted at treatment of angina but failed to increase cardiac blood flow while unexpectedly increasing blood flow to the penis, and sleep-inducing drugs that exploit the roles of gamma aminobutyric acid (GABA) and melatonin receptors in brain sleep systems. Recent news stories on topics ranging from brain systems and drugs for memory enhancement (Rovner, 2007; Foer, 2007), appetite control (Bentivoglio and Kristensson, 2007), and sleep drugs (Saul, 2007) reflect the intense public, academic, and commercial interest in neuropsychopharmacology.

This interpretation of neuroscience research carries the additional implication that drugs can achieve or modulate not only abnormal, diseased, or disordered psychology but also normal, healthy, or optimal function. Development and utilization of drugs to treat psychiatric disorders have been accompanied by important changes in expectations of physicians, consumers, and policy makers about how drugs can and should be used (Barrett, 2007; Chatterjee, 2007; Kelly, 2007). These changes include expectations about the duration of a drug’s use by an individual and who drives the choice of whether, when, and which drugs to use; opinions on personal drug use; and ideas about which human functions can, or should, be modulated by drug use. It may be particularly important that many current prescription drugs, and possibly the majority of current prescription drugs with primarily psychological effects, are used widely off-label. 6 Such off-label use and the possibility of important placebo and nocebo effects (Lasagna et al., 1954, Beecher, 1955; Olshansky, 2007) cannot but open analysts’ eyes to the probability that drugs have heretofore undiscovered effects.

Drugs that are described and marketed as having disease-specific or diagnosis-specific effects are quite capable of producing striking psychological effects in individuals who do not suffer from the condition identified with the drug’s descriptive name or clinical indication. These psychological effects may or may not be similar to the effects in individuals with medical conditions for which the drugs are prescribed. Recent examples discussed in the public press include use of antipsychotic drugs for individuals without psychosis, beta-blockers to modulate performance anxiety in concert musicians, and steroids to enhance athletic performance. Other emerging changes in how drugs are prescribed and used include increased incidences of polypharmacy, of consumer decisions about whether and when to use drugs (as illustrated by erectile dysfunction drugs and Botox), and of consumer decisions to seek out and use available drugs for multiple effects. For example, the use of certain prescription drugs as study aids is not uncommon (White et al., 2006). Of 1,025 U.S. college students surveyed, 16.2 percent reported such use of prescription stimulants. Ninety percent of this group did not hold a legitimate prescription for stimulant drugs; 96 percent of them used Ritalin to improve attention, improve ability to participate in partying, reduce hyperactivity, and improve grades; and 15.5 percent reported using Ritalin at least two or three times per week.

Addiction and substance (or drug) abuse presents another area of change. The demarcation between abused drugs and medicines is situation dependent, as demonstrated by recent experience with OxyContin, by nicotine in over-the-counter smoking cessation aids, and by college-student exploitation of prescription stimulants. The illicit market not only provides incentives for novel drugs, manufacturing processes, and delivery systems but also poses novel risks. These factors are illustrated by episodes such as the appearance of permanent symptoms of Parkinson’s disease in opiate addicts who used the illicitly manufactured 1-methyl-4-phenyl-1,2,3,6-tetrahydropyridine (MPTP), a neurotoxin probably produced by improper chemical conditions in illicit manufacture of meperidine, a synthetic opioid (Langston, 1995). The incentives provided by the illicit market are also exemplified by recent U.S. experience with methamphetamine labs and by the emergence of crack cocaine (smoked, minimal manufacturing danger, sold in small quantities) to challenge freebase cocaine (smoked, risks of explosion during manufacture), both of which challenged powdered cocaine (snorted or injected, sold in large quantities) (Kleiman, 1992; Musto, 1987). Epidemio logical and clinical research on the natural history of drug abuse also provides information about the long-term effects (often) high doses of at least a subset of performance-altering drugs, which may alert analysts to dual-use possibilities. The same may be true of patterns of drug use in highly competitive industries or markets, such as the steroids, stimulants, and endurance-altering drugs used by athletes or the stimulants used by floor traders.

Cognition Enhancers

One specific area of neuropsychopharmacology that may be of considerable importance is that of cognition enhancement. Cognition enhancers can be broadly defined as drugs or other agents that have the potential to improve human functions such as attention, learning, and memory (Sarter, 2006; Sahakian and Morein-Samir, 2007 and accompanying online discussion at http://network.nature.com/forums/naturenewsandopinion/ ). Considerable research investment in the United States and other countries is directed to the discovery and development of pharmacological cognition enhancers. Often, these agents target declines in memory and cognition linked to age, dementia, or neuropsychiatric or neurological disease. It is likely, however, that agents developed for prevention or treatment of disease will alter brain processes in normal individuals. Additionally, agents already in wide medical or social use are known to alter memory and cognition, sometimes by mechanisms shared with disease-related agents and sometimes by mechanisms not so clearly linked to age or pathology. Examples of widely used agents with known or suspected ability to alter and perhaps enhance certain components of human cognition and/or performance include caffeine (found in coffee, tea, soda), nicotine (tobacco products), tacrine (energy drinks), amphetamines and methylphenidate (attention deficit hyperactivity disorder medications and certain abused drugs), propranolol (certain beta-blockers), dextroamphetamine, and modafinil.

A fairly wide range of neuropharmacological or chemical systems have been suggested as possible sources for cognition or performance enhancers (e.g., Sarter, 2006). This expanding list is illustrative of the wide range of brain processes suspected of having potential to enhance normal and/or disordered cognition. Conversely, opposite changes or disruption in such systems might disrupt cognition and/or performance. Specifically, if agonists of a particular system enhance cognition, it is mechanistically plausible that antagonists might disrupt cognition; conversely, if antagonists of a particular neurotransmitter enhance, its agonists might disrupt. Examples of the former might include dopamine agonists, which enhance attention, and dopamine antagonists, which disrupt it; examples of the latter might include the suspected cognitive enhancing effects of cannabinoid antagonists and the disrupting effects of agonists like THC.

Three areas of research in behavioral pharmacology have particular importance for analysis of enhancement and/or disruption of cognition and performance. One identifies the boundaries or limitations of cognition enhancement. Many current models of cognition incorporate ideas of processing resources and their limitations. It is an open question as to whether drug-induced enhancements in one area of cognition have a cost in other areas. Stimulant drugs are known to have rate-dependent effects, such that the same exposure regimen may enhance attentional processes that are initially occurring with low frequency but simultaneously decrease attentional processes that are occurring at high frequency (Dews and Wenger, 1977), which could result in unwanted performance decrements. A cognition enhancer that optimizes performance under a taxing attention condition might not improve performance of a task with lower attention demands (Robbins, 2005) and in fact might disrupt it. As another example, drugs that enhance working memory capacity or operation may impair capacity to simultaneously filter distractors; this could increase false alarms in a detection operation (Lavie, 2005). Public recognition that modafinil, which promotes wakefulness and increases alertness, can have small but valuable cognitive effects is an example of a change that creates opportunities for public discussion of the risks and benefits of potential cognitive enhancers.

A second relevant area of behavioral pharmacology involves efforts to identify which specific neurochemical changes actually covary with cognitive processes. Studies of neurochemistry in cholinergic brain systems, for example, suggest that drug-induced changes in neurotransmitter release activated by attentional processes are vitally different from drug-induced changes in basal release (Sarter and Bruno, 1994; Kozak et al., 2006).

A third, and related, area is research to identify agents that produce specific kinds of cognitive enhancements rather than broadly acting agents (Rovner, 2007). Additionally, the full range of effects of potential cognition- or performance-enhancing drugs warrants attention. For example, stimulants such as amphetamine can increase attention and concentration, but they also exert cardiovascular effects that can be exacerbated by physical exertion and heat, and with prolonged or high dose use, they carry risks of addiction and paranoia.

Implications for Agents That May Act to Change or Disrupt Various Aspects of Human Psychology

There is currently a widely recognized “translational gap” between preclinical research, clinical research, and development of cognition enhancers, perhaps best articulated in the area of schizophrenia (Hagan and Jones, 2005; Floresco et al., 2005). Similar translational gaps exist in most, if not all, areas of neuropsychopharmacology. Examples of recognized roadblocks to new therapeutic entities include the need for improved clinical models, the rudimentary knowledge of brain neurochemistry and function, the paucity of models to predict side effects, and poor understanding of brain diseases and disorders. One recommendation for closing these translational gaps is to improve the predictive power of animal models so that they map onto operationally defined domains of affective, cogni tive, and motivational processes (Robbins, 1998; Sarter, 2006; Barrett, 2007). The neuronal bases of cognitive function are poorly understood, and most animal models that are used to identify potentially useful therapeutics are not based on molecular- or systems-level understanding of brain processes or on functional understanding of human cognition and behavior. Improved animal models for human psychopathology (depression, anxiety, memory decline, cognitive failure) and for normal functioning (learning, affect, motivation, performance) could permit development of novel drugs and/or drugs that can be targeted to effect human cognition and performance. While animal models are useful, many effects on cognitive dimensions cannot really be tested on rats or mice.

The discovery of the probable role of the hypocretins (also called orexins) in human narcolepsy shows that once the chemistry is known and the market drivers are in place, a drug can be developed rapidly. Symptoms of narcolepsy are currently managed with amphetamine-like CNS stimulants or modafinil (for excessive daytime sleepiness) and antidepressants or sodium oxylate (for cataplexy), but none of these treatments are based on an understanding of how brain chemistry is dysregulated in affected individuals. Recent research, which arose from the discovery of narcolepsy genes in animal models, suggests that a deficiency of hypocretin/orexim is responsible for about 90 percent of human narcolepsy-cataplexy cases (Nishino, 2007a,b). This finding led directly to development of new diagnostic tests for narcolepsy and has led to the search for new therapeutic drugs for narcolepsy caused by a deficiency of hypocretin/orexim. Additionally, novel small-molecule hypocretin/orexim receptor antagonists that can be used to inhibit feeding have been identified.

Finding 2-3. Neurochemical systems modulate, and can be used to control, a wide range of human psychology. The number of neuropsychopharmacological drugs increased dramatically after the mid-1900s, along with their availability, and emerging technologies may improve the ability to harness drug effects or to produce targeted changes in human psychology. Cognitive neuroscientists do not have specific understanding of how most drugs produce their effects. Basic research in the public or private sectors that identifies the specific mechanisms of disease and of drug effects might enable rapid development of new drugs. New drugs may have unrecognized effects that emerge owing to variation in individuals, settings, or performance demands.

Nanotechnology in Medicine

In addition to bringing new drug entities or new uses for existing entities, emerging technologies might allow new pathways for drug delivery. Some observers say it is likely that the paradigm of the pharmaceutical industry will change, from “discovering” drugs by screening many compounds to the purposeful engineering of desired molecules.

Richard Feynman famously said, “There’s plenty of room at the bottom,” in a lecture in which he outlined the principle of manipulating individual atoms using larger machines to manufacture increasingly smaller machines (Feynman, 1959). Nanotechnology is a rapidly expanding, multidisciplinary field that applies engineering and manufacturing principles at a molecular level. It can be roughly divided into categories that include nanobiotechnology, biological microelectro-mechanical systems, microfluidics, biosensors, microarrays, and tissue micro-engineering (Gourley, 2005). In some sense nanotechnology is intuitive, since everything in nature is built upward from the atomic level in order to define limits and structures (Emerich and Thanos, 2006). Understanding and developing nanotechnology, therefore, depends on understanding these limits and pushing against them.

Nanomedicine (the development of effective clinical treatments based on nanotechnology) has had some successes (Freitas, 1999) and depends on several overlapping molecular technologies. These new but progressing technologies include (1) the construction of nanoscale-sized structures for diagnostics, biosensors, and local drug delivery; (2) genomics, proteomics, and nanoengineered microbes; and (3) the creation of molecular machines capable of identifying and eliminating host pathogens by replacing and repairing cells and cellular components in vivo (Emerich and Thanos, 2006). Of particular importance may be nanotechnologies that allow delivery of drugs across the blood-brain barrier in ways now impossible.

Nanotechnology for Drug Delivery

In the last decade nanotechnology and nanofabrication have significantly impacted the field of drug delivery (Emerich and Thanos, 2006). The continued development of these technologies will probably occur in conjunction with the development of pharmaceuticals targeted at the brain (Ellis-Behnke et al., 2007; Jain, 2007; Koo et al., 2006; Silva, 2006, 2007; Suri et al., 2007; Teixido and Giralt, 2008). The development of such neuropharmaceutical combinations is of great interest to the Department of Defense. Guided by the statement of task, the committee outlines in the following paragraphs nanotechnologies that may contribute to advances in the delivery of CNS drugs.

Techniques have shifted from microfabrication and micromachining (e.g., the osmotic pump) to designs ranging from secondary constructs at the nanometer scale (e.g., microspheres). The engineering of nano delivery systems for small molecules, proteins, and DNA has led to the emergence of entirely new and previously unpredicted fields. Formulation science has linked up with computer technology to create a controlled-release microchip capable of infinite modulation that would allow for the greatly improved controlled release of pharmaceutical agents (Santini et al., 1999; Grayson et al., 2003; Maloney et al., 2005; Prescott et al., 2006). Tissue engineering applications have also moved toward the development and implementation of nanometer-sized components. The creation of artificial cells with appropriate physiologic properties may provide a better understanding of normal physiological processes. Transfection systems on the nanoscale for genetic manipulation and gene delivery are being tailored using different polymers.

Nanotechnology is opening new therapeutic opportunities for agents that could not be used effectively as conventional drug formulations owing to their poor bioavailability or drug instability (Santini et al., 1999). Microsphere formulations are used to protect agents susceptible to degradation or denaturation while prolonging their duration of action by increasing systemic exposure or retention of the formulation (Hillyer and Albrecht, 2001; Hussain et al., 2001; Torche et al., 2000; Van Der Lubben et al., 2001; Varde and Pack, 2004). Nanoparticles are able to cross membrane barriers, particularly in the absorptive epithelium of the small intestine (Hillyer and Albrecht, 2001) and are being used to deliver small molecules, proteins, and other therapeutics (Dunning et al., 2004; Hamaguchi et al., 2005; Koushik et al., 2004; Panyam and Labhasetwar, 2003; Silva, 2006; Weissleder et al., 2005). Biodegradable nanospheres enhance bioavailability through uptake, followed by degradation and disappearance of the vehicle from the system.

Integration of controlled-release drug reservoirs with microchips (Santini et al., 1999) provides unlimited potential for modulating drug release. Nanotubes that have large relative internal volumes also can be functionalized on the inside surface (Martin and Kohli, 2003). One fabrication technique used self-assembling lipid microtubes to deliver testosterone in rats (Goldstein et al., 2001). Testosterone was covalently bound with an ester linkage to a glutamide core lipid, forming nanotubes that possessed an in vivo biphasic release profile characterized by an initial burst followed by a more sustained release. Another method of fabrication involves synthesizing carbon nanotubes using fullerene. These nanotubes range from one nanometer to tens of nanometers in diameter and are from several to hundreds of microns long. Drugs can be covalently attached to functional groups on the external surface of the nanotubes (Chen et al., 2001). Another drug delivery approach uses nanoshells or dielectric-metal (gold-coated silica) nanospheres. When embedded in a drug-containing polymer and then injected into the body, these nanoshells accumulate near tumor cells. When heated with an infrared laser, the polymer melts, releasing the drug at a specific site (Hirsch et al., 2005; Loo et al., 2005). This technology allows delivering drugs at very precise locations in the brain. Growing knowledge about the neural circuits underlying various functions will probably enhance the capacity to target specific effects with fewer side effects.

Improvements in drug-containing nanoparticles are already gaining regulatory approval. Abraxane, a nanoparticle form of albumin and paclitaxel, 7 elimi nates the need for toxic solvents in earlier versions of paclitaxel (Taxol) and permits more of the drug to be administered. Similarly, a micellar nanoparticle formulation of paciltaxel (NK105) is being developed to reduce toxicity while enhancing antitumor activity (Hamaguchi et al., 2005). There is broad international interest in research on nanotechnology for drug delivery. Asia, in particular, is active in this area, as evidenced by the published literature (Bosi et al., 2000; Miyata and Yamakoshi, 1997; Chen et al., 2001; Tabata et al., 1997a,b; Tsao et al., 1999, 2001; Hamaguchi et al., 2005), and there is especially strong research in drug delivery to the brain and neuropeptides (Gao et al., 2007a,b). 8

Finding 2-4. Technological advances will affect the types of neuropsychopharmacological drugs available and methods for drug delivery. For the IC, nanotechnologies that allow drugs to cross the blood-brain barrier, increase the precision of delivery, evade immune system defenses, evade metabolism, or prolong actions at cellular or downstream targets will be of particular importance. These technologies will increase the likelihood that various peptides, or other brain proteins, could ultimately be utilized as drugs. Development of antidotes or protective agents against various classes of drugs that could be used by an enemy force will also be important.

Neuropeptides and Behavior

Neuropeptides act as messengers in the brain, influencing many neurobehavioral functions (Strand, 1999). Their therapeutic use in humans has been hampered because they do not readily pass the blood-brain barrier (BBB) and they induce potent hormonelike side effects in the blood (Illum, 2000; Pardridge, 1999). To date, the results of intranasal administration testing have been mixed (Born et al., 2002; Heinrichs et al., 2003; Heinrichs et al., 2004; Merkus and van den Berg, 2007). However, nanotechnology may someday allow for quick pharmacological modifications of behaviors. Box 2-2 provides an overview of the function of the BBB.

The Blood-Brain Barrier as an Obstacle to the Delivery of Therapeutics. The blood-brain barrier (BBB) remains an obstacle to the delivery of therapeutics to the brain. It comprises an endothelial cell monolayer associated with pericytes and astrocytes. (more...)

One neuropeptide of interest is oxytocin, which—in addition to its well-known functions in milk letdown and childbirth—has a central role in positive social behavioral interactions and can increase trust behavior in human experimental subjects. Oxytocin receptors are distributed in brain regions associated with certain behaviors (Huber et al., 2005; Landgraf and Neumann, 2004) such as pair bonding, maternal care, sexual behavior, and the ability to form normal social attachments (Carter, 1998; Carter et al., 2001; Heinrichs et al., 2002; Huber et al., 2005; Insel and Young, 2001; Pedersen, 1997; Uvnäs-Moberg, 1998; Young et al., 2001). Thus oxytocin permits animals to overcome their natural avoidance of proximity and facilitates approach behavior. Given that oxytocin is believed to promote social attachment and affiliation in nonhuman mammals, researchers have hypothesized it might also promote more social behaviors—such as trust—in humans (Kosfeld et al., 2005; Zak et al., 2007; Zak and Fakhar, 2006).

  • FUNCTIONAL NEUROIMAGING

Introduction

Broadly defined, functional neuroimaging is the use of neuroimaging technology to measure aspects of brain function, often with the goal of understanding the relationship between regional brain activity and specific tasks, stimuli, cognition, behaviors or neural processes. Common technologies for functional neuroimaging include multichannel electroencephalography (EEG), magnetoencephalography (MEG), positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), functional transcranial Doppler sonography (fTCDS), and magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS). EEG and MEG measure localized electrical or magnetic fluctuations in neuronal activity. PET, fMRI, fNIRS, and fTCDS can measure localized changes in cerebral blood flow related to neural activity. PET and MRS can measure regional modulation of brain metabolism and neurochemistry in response to neural activity or processes. These functional neuroimaging technologies are complementary, and each offers a different window onto complex neural processes. Because of this complementarity, multimodal imaging is an emerging area of great interest for research, clinical, commercial, and defense applications.

Neuroimaging technologies are likely to play an important role in endeavors to enhance cognition as well as affect and motivation over the next two decades. Predictions about future applications of technology are always speculative, but if the issues discussed above come to fruition this emergent technology might provide insight into the following areas, and others, of direct relevance to national defense: the acquisition of intelligence from captured unlawful combatants, enhanced training techniques, augmented cognition and memory enhancement of soldiers and intelligence operatives, the screening of terrorism suspects at checkpoints or ports of entry where there are no constitutional protections (i.e., airports), and soldier-machine interface devices such as are used in remotely piloted vehicles and prosthetics. Indeed, science is already beginning to see contributions of this field to clinical and battlefield medicine.

Over the next two decades, good brain-computer interfaces (BCIs) are likely to be a great of interest to the gaming industry as well as to the rehabilitation, medical, and military sectors, and neuroimaging and neurophysiology will play a central role in those endeavors. BCIs are likely to be used to enhance several areas of cognition, including memory, concentration, and emotional intelligence, among others. Indeed, this has been a focus of the Defense Advance Research Projects Agency’s (DARPA’s) Augmented Cognition program for several years. Brain prosthetics could become the new input-output devices for memory systems, allowing efficient searching and encyclopedic access to information. Sandberg and Bostrom suggest BCIs may improve better concentration by reducing working memory load and exploiting the broad attention abilities of the visual system.

Similarly, DARPA has been working on EEG and fNIRS-based BCIs that use the human visual system as the input device to a computer system to increase the speed of data processing in visual search mode. The idea is that although current technological capacities with computer vision are not even close to the speed and sophistication of the human eye, there is a lag time between the visual mental process and a motor output to the computer system. By using EEG or fNIRS to directly measure the brain’s response when it detects a target, the search process can occur more quickly than with an operator’s motor response. Given the current level of miniaturization of computer memory, a wearable computer with an efficient brain-based input/output device (a “mental mouse”) and an efficient search strategy could allow access to huge amounts of stored data in milliseconds, effectively augmenting the user’s long-term memory. Such devices could allow military troops to access visual maps and intelligence when they approach a new area or to access medical information in the field.

Sandberg and Bostrom suggest that wearable computers could also enhance emotional intelligence—that is, the ability to perceive emotions in others and respond appropriately. Such capabilities could be useful for gathering intelligence. The current prototype system was designed to help people who have difficulty in accurately assessing the emotions of other people—for example, children with Asperger syndrome—by improving their ability to interact (El Kaliouby and Robinson, 2005). The system consists of a camera to record the facial expressions of a conversational partner, facial and emotion processing software that estimates the most likely emotional state, a readout that displays a cartoon of the emotion, and suggestions for a proper response. Such systems could also help a participant in a high-stakes negotiation or interview to gauge more accurately the emotional intentions of his or her counterpart (see, for example, Ekman and O’Sullivan, 2006). Indeed, Ekman and Mark Frank (Frank and Ekman, 2004), along with others at DARPA, have been using computer-aided analysis of facial responses based on Ekman’s theories to study at-a-distance measures of deception.

Owing to their awkward size and shape and their cost, some technologies such as MEG and MRI/fMRI are likely to take a backseat in enabling cognitive enhancement, providing the underlying neuroscience upon which more fieldable technologies, such as EEG and fNIRS, can be based. It is, for example, unlikely that brain–computer interfaces based on fMRI could be loaded onto jets or spaceships in the next two decades, whereas an EEG or fNIRS-based system could very well be so deployed in that timeframe. However, the information about cognitive, affective, and motivational states that accrues from these various kinds of neuroimaging is likely to play a key role in the use of technology-based systems to enhance performance.

Another example is the use of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to facilitate neural changes that have been identified through neuroimaging. TMS is a noninvasive way to excite neurons in the brain: rapidly changing magnetic fields (electromagnetic induction) are used to induce weak electric currents in neural tissue, allowing the brain to be activated from outside with minimal discomfort. One alternative to using pharmacological agents to influence neural function is to use electrical stimulation such as TMS to excite the neuromodulatory centers that control plasticity. Experiments in the monkey have shown that electrical stimulation can result in faster cortical reorganization. In their review, Sandberg and Bostrom cite evidence that TMS can increase or decrease the excitability of the cortex, in turn changing its level of plasticity. TMS has been used to facilitate the learning of a procedural memory task by stimulating the motor cortex. Sandberg and Bostrom suggest TMS has succeeded in facilitating working memory, classification, learning motor skills such as finger tapping sequences, coordinating visuomotor tasks, and consolidating declarative memory during sleep.

Because TMS is noninvasive and able to improve performance on a variety of cognitive tasks, Sandberg and Bostrom suggest that it could be a very versatile tool for cognitive enhancement. One limitation is the short duration of its effect (minutes to an hour after stimulation), although some results suggest that coupling TMS with pharmacological manipulations of the dopaminergic system could facilitate long-term consolidation or longer effects TMS (Nitsche et al., 2006). It is worthwhile reminding the reader that the considerable interindividual variability in responses to TMS might require individual tuning of dosage, placement, and so on. Neuroimaging tools such as MRI, fMRI, or fNIRS could play a role in providing precise localization for such technology integrations.These functional neuroimaging technologies, some mature, others emergent, are commonplace in research and clinical environments and are having an impact on defense policy decisions (Peters et al., 2008). Recent advances and developments allow for functional neuroimaging capability with real-time or near-real-time data acquisition and analysis that is becoming cheaper, portable, and more user friendly. Continued refinement of these technologies is likely to lead to increased dissemination of this technology, with applications expanding well beyond the current primary fields of neuroscience research and clinical medicine. Areas where the application of advanced functional neuroimaging technology likely are business (marketing, economics, human resources), human performance, risk assessment, the field of law, and the military, all of them having great relevance to national policy and defense issues. Progress continues to be made in both functional neuroimaging and neurophysiological methods toward the holy grail of neuroimaging, namely, millisecond-level temporal resolution with precise spatial localization. No current technology affords both high temporal resolution and high spatial resolution with access to the full brain. The strengths and limitations of each technology are briefly described below.

Electroencephalography

The oldest device used to assess brain function in real time is the electroencephalograph. Many years ago it became known that electrical activity in the brain could be recorded by placing electrodes on the surface of the scalp (Berger, 1929). Such recordings represent the summated electrical signal from nominally 50,000 local neurons. Early studies focused on the spontaneous rhythmic oscillations in voltage—frequency bands that tended to shift together with changing mental status, such as alpha waves, which have frequencies between 8 and 13 Hz. Early clinical EEG was used primarily to detect and diagnose epilepsy, but today, with advances in computer technology, informative new experimental paradigms and techniques are being developed. Electroencephalographic recordings are of two main types: continuous and discrete. Continuous recordings are the traditional multitrace waveforms recorded since EEGs began and activity is classified by the frequency of the dominant waveform (0-40 Hz) on any given channel, such as alpha waves. Discrete recordings are triggered by an event, such as an external flash of light, and then the next 1 to 4 seconds of activity are recorded. In discrete recordings, the “normal” EEG waves are considered background.

Discrete, or event-related, recordings were first described by Davis (1939), who noticed that event-related changes could be seen in an ongoing electroencephalogram event-related potentials (ERPs), currently the focus of electroencephalographic research, refer to the measurement of the brain’s electrophysiological response to a particular stimulus. The brain’s response to discrete stimuli are typically relatively small (a few microvolts) compared with the ongoing background EEG activity (approximately 50 mV), and multiple stimulus presentations are averaged to distinguish the response associated with the stimulus from the background activity. When the brain response is largely automatic and dictated by the physical properties of the stimulus (say, the loudness of a sound or the brightness of a light flash) it is called an evoked potential. Evoked potentials generally occur 15-100 ms after a stimulus is presented. Later responses, which occur as early as 150 ms after a stimulus, are thought to be influenced by cognitive processes and are referred to as ERPs.

Quantitative electroencephalography (QEEG) uses postrecording computer analysis to analyze the relationship between each of the electrodes placed at the scalp. The frequency composition, amplitude, and position of each electrode is compared to the same information taken from a database of individuals without any known neurological disorder. The resulting EEG brain maps are then analyzed with sophisticated statistical techniques to reveal patterns. The results of these analyses can be presented in graphical form as topographical displays of brain electrical activity. Applications include neurofeedback, or neurotherapy, and the identification of responses to medication for certain neurological and psychiatric disorders. Neurotherapy is an experimental technique that uses a QEEG brain map to analyze psychiatric problems from attention deficit disorder to depression to schizophrenia. Patients are then subjected to a conditioning protocol to “train” the “abnormal” brain activity toward a statistically more “normal” pattern of activity. Neurotherapy can reduce aberrant symptoms of many conditions (Fox et al., 2005).

Interpretation of a scalp electroencephalogram often involves speculation as to the location inside the brain of the source of the activity recorded (Brazier, 1949; Shaw and Roth, 1955). While there is substantial PET and fMRI literature on finding the neural sources of the functional network implicated in given mental tasks (Cabeza and Nyberg, 2000), PET and fMRI are temporally limited in their ability to probe discrete neural events. The source localization capability of EEG has been used to overcome this limitation and solve the inverse problem. Even though EEG offers millisecond-level time resolution, the signals measured at the scalp do not directly indicate the location of the neurons that are generating the activity. Although the sites at which the scalp potentials are measured at any given point in time are finite, an infinite number of source configurations could account for those measurements (Plonsey, 1963; Fender, 1987). Source localization involves mathematical attempts to solve the inverse problem by introducing a priori assumptions about the generation of the EEG (or MEG) signals. The better these assumptions are, the more trustworthy the source estimations will be, and several different models have been formulated and implemented in algorithms to reach the inverse solution, each using different mathematical, biophysical, statistical, anatomical, or functional constraints (for a recent review, see Michel et al., 2004). Technological advances in the field include noncontact electrodes that use high-gain preamplifiers to mitigate the effects of the high impedence caused by the lack of contact. This arrangement could allow the “application” of a large number of electrodes in a relatively short time, at the cost of a noisier signal. As these models and constraint estimates improve, there is promise for important future developments. EEG has several advantages over other functional neuroimaging techniques, including the relatively low cost of the technology (around $15 million). Also, a single technician can produce reliable recordings with unmatched temporal resolution measured in milliseconds. A number of other countries use high-density EEG with source localization. 9

Positron Emission Tomography

The introduction of computed tomography (CT) by Sir Godfrey Hounsfield in 1973 (Petrik et al., 2006) dramatically changed the way scientists and physicians examined the brain. The development of PET (based on prior brain autoradiographic work) quickly followed, creating in vivo autoradiograms of brain function (Ter-Pogossian et al., 1975; Phelps et al., 1975) and introducing a new era of functional brain mapping. For an excellent historical review of PET and functional neuroimaging the reader is referred to Raichle (1998).

PET can be used to produce a three-dimensional image or map of functional processes in the brain. The system detects pairs of gamma rays emitted indirectly by a positron-emitting radioisotope, which is introduced into the body on a metabolically active molecule. Images of regional metabolic activity or blood flow are then reconstructed by computer analysis. Modern versions of PET scanners are combined with CT scanning and MRI scanning capability to coregister metabolic activity with high-resolution anatomic images of the brain, creating three-dimensional metabolic/anatomic overlays.

The radionuclides utilized in PET scanning typically have short half lives; carbon-11 (~20 min), nitrogen-13 (~10 min), oxygen-15 (123), and fluorine-18 (~110 min). They are incorporated into compounds such as glucose and water. These radiotracers distribute in the brain by following the metabolic pathways of their natural analogues or by binding with specificity to the receptor proteins for which they have affinity. Due to the short half lives of most radioisotopes, the radiotracers must be produced in a cyclotron and a certified medicinal radiochemistry laboratory co-located with the PET facility. Fluorine-18, with a half life long enough to allow commercial manufacture at an offsite location and transport to an imaging center daily, is an exception.

PET has gained widespread utility in clinical medicine, particularly in oncology, where it has become the favored imaging technology for the detection, staging, and monitoring of response to treatment for many neoplasms. Clinical PET is also used in neurology, psychiatry, cardiology, and pharmacology. There is continued widespread use of PET technology to study brain metabolism and receptor ligands. Limitations of PET include its relatively low temporal resolution; spatial resolution limited to approximately 5 mm; relatively expensive equipment; requirement for an injectable, short-lived positron-emitting radioisotope that is usually produced in a cyclotron; and limits on its use for repetitive longitudinal studies and studies in certain populations owing to its emission of ionizing radiation.

However, PET remains a powerful tool for functional neuroimaging, especially with the proliferation of PET/CT and PET/MRI scanners. PET’s exquisite ability to elucidate specific receptor binding sites/activity within the brain and its ability to produce images of brain metabolism mean it is not likely to be supplanted by other neuroimaging technologies in the foreseeable future. Indeed molecular neuroimaging via PET is likely to show the most growth in functional neuroimaging research over the next decade (Hammound et al., 2007).

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

MRI is widely accepted as the gold standard for anatomical neuroimaging. The most common form of functional MRI (fMRI) utilizes a blood-oxygenation-level-dependent (BOLD) contrast mechanism to distinguish areas of neural activity. Other methodologies for fMRI include dynamic contrast techniques and noncontrast techniques (e.g., arterial spin labeling). There has been explosive growth of fMRI research and clinical applications over the past decade, with research applications including brain mapping of task (motor and cognitive) dependent processes. MRI has also been employed for detection of deception, an application that has drawn the interest of various communities (ethics, defense, legal). Under controlled experimental conditions with cooperative subjects, this technology has shown initial promise (Abe et al., 2006, 2007; Mohamed et al., 2006; Davatzikos et al., 2005; Kozel et al., 2004a,b, 2005; Langleben et al., 2002, 2005; Lee et al., 2002, 2005; Nuñez et al., 2005; Phan et al., 2005a; Ganis et al., 2003; Spence et al., 2001).

Real-time data acquisition (single-trial fMRI) and near-real-time data analysis (hundreds of milliseconds delay) of complex cognitive tasks have been demonstrated and will expand the applications areas of relevance to the research, clinical, and defense communities (Posse et al., 2003; Phan et al., 2004). Real-time fMRI has been utilized to demonstrate the voluntary suppression of affective state, suggesting that it may provide insight into complex cognitive processes (Phan et al., 2005b). Whether these findings can be generalized to nonexperimental settings remains to be determined. fMRI has many advantages over other functional imaging techniques, including high spatial resolution of the activation patterns (measured in millimeters); temporal resolution (measured in a few seconds); no known risk factors in healthy subjects; 10 and, recently near-real-time analysis. Its key disadvantages include its relatively high cost; problems with data interpretation if the subject moves a few millimeters; a user-unfriendly scanning environment (noisy, small enclosed space); and the requirement for large super-conducting magnets.

Recent advances in neuroimaging technology, including high-field (3 tesla) and ultrahigh-field (7 tesla) magnetic resonance techniques for MRI, fMRI and MRS, real-time acquisition/processing, and parallel imaging, offer the potential for significant advances in spatial and temporal resolution for structural, functional and neurochemical imaging. In other words, MRI-based imaging technologies are providing faster and more detailed pictures of the human brain and brain function than ever before. These and other related technologies are moving forward rapidly, driven by clinical and research demand, and over the next two decades there are likely to be continuing significant advances in this technology, with unique applications certain to emerge (Dickerson, 2007; Ladd, 2007; Nakada, 2007).

As shown in Table 2-3 , a concerted effort at the national level in China to invest in research relating to high-field structural and functional fMRI has led to a network of coordinated laboratories and programs (Cao et al., 2006; Poo and Guo, 2007; Simon, 2007). 11

TABLE 2-3. Current Neuroimaging Research in the People’s Republic of China.

Current Neuroimaging Research in the People’s Republic of China.

Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy

Magnetic resonance spectroscopy (MRS) provides a noninvasive window into brain chemistry. Research clinical applications include the ability to differentiate pathology (e.g., brain neoplasm) from normal or necrotic tissue (Moore, 1998); monitoring brain metabolism (glutamate, glucose levels); and monitoring neuropharmacologic treatment effects (neurotropic/neuroprotective medicines or psychoactive compounds) (Manji et al., 1999; Moore et al., 2000). While spatial resolution is on the order of 1 cc, this methodology has the potential to monitor neurochemical modulation in response to neural processes or neuropharmacologic intervention. Multiple studies have demonstrated the ability of MRS to detect biomarkers of complex neural processes, and rapid (<30 sec) neurochemical imaging becomes possible with high-field magnetic resonance technology (Phan et al., 2005c). Advances in MRS should be followed carefully because its complementarity with respect to the other functional neuroimaging technologies, particularly in the area of monitoring neuropharmacologic response, is likely to make it applicable for defense purposes. For example, as the ability to monitor neurochemistry in vivo and in near real time is developed with advanced high-field MRS and related technologies, the possibility arises of developing state-dependent neurochemical biomarkers for stress and anxiety as well as their pharmacologic modulation in a dose response fashion.

Magnetoelectroencephalography

Magnetoencephalography (MEG) is a completely noninvasive, nonhazardous technology for functional brain mapping, localizing and characterizing the electrical activity of the CNS by measuring the associated magnetic fields emanating from the brain. Every electrical current generates a magnetic field. However, unlike an electrical signal, magnetic fields are not distorted by traveling through the skull, and the source of the summated magnetic fields can be triangulated within a few millimeters. MEG provides functional mapping information on the working brain.

Modern MEG scanners use as many as 300 superconducting quantum interference device (SQUID) 12 detectors, allowing very fast acquisition and extremely high localization of the source of the electromagnetic signal. The information provided by MEG is entirely different from but complementary to the information provided by structural imaging techniques like CT or MR imaging. While MRI and CT provide excellent anatomical images, MEG measures correlates of neurological function. The advantages of MEG over fMRI and PET include the measurement of brain activity with higher temporal and spatial resolution. Its disadvantages include its greater cost than fMRI. It also requires a specialized technical team with broad expertise in the acquisition and processing of complex data and requires very precise positioning requirements.

Transcranial Ultrasonography

While transcranial ultrasonography operates on the same principle as the diagnostic ultrasound imaging of a fetus in utero, it is more difficult to obtain high-quality images of the brain because the propagation of sound waves is impaired by bone. However, the skull is thin enough in a few “monographic windows” (Duscheck and Schandry, 2003) to provide a path for the ultrasonic signal and can provide accurate real-time measurements of blood flow velocity. The transorbital window, located above the zygomatic arch (the “temple”), is used to image the posterior, anterior, and medial cerebral arteries along with a few of the branches that provide blood flow to specific areas of the brain.

Although both rely on blood flow, sonography is very different from fMRI, which measures blood oxygenation level changes with a spatial resolution of a couple of millimeters. In functional transcranial doppler sonography (fTDS), the spatial resolution is determined by the volume of the brain supplied with blood by the vessel under study. These areas can be quite large, making the spatial resolution of fTDS extremely limited. Changes in blood velocity, which are presumed to directly measure changes in resistance of the artery (i.e., the lumen diameter change), occur nearly instantaneously in an event- related experimental paradigm, giving exceptional temporal resolution.

There remain several technical problems with fTDS. Only a limited number of large arteries can be imaged. Even in the arteries that are large enough and located within sight of the few available ultrasonic windows, the angle of the ultrasonic beam can make it very difficult to accurately measure blood flow changes. However, fTDS has several advantages over other functional neuroimaging techniques including cost effectiveness; portability; continuous monitoring of blood flow activity; and excellent temporal resolution.

Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy

Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is an emerging neuroimaging technology with several characteristics that make it a good candidate for use in military and intelligence applications. fNIRS uses light in the near infrared (700-900 nm), outside the visible spectrum, to measure changes in brain tissue that are associated with neuronal activity—in other words, it provides accurate spatial information about ongoing brain activity. Although fNIRS can measure several parameters associated with neural activity, the most common is the change in the ratio of oxygenated to deoxygenated hemoglobin in the blood, a measure analogous to fMRI’s BOLD signal. Data from Huppert et al. (2006) demonstrate the relationship between the fMRI BOLD signal and fNIRS measures of deoxyhemoglobin (HbR), oxyhemoglobin (HbO), and total hemoglobin (HbT) ( Figure 2-3 ). The design used a short-duration, event-related motor task, finger tapping, during the simultaneous recording of fMRI and fNIRS in five subjects. The results of Huppert et al. indicate that the fMRI-measured BOLD response is more highly correlated with the fNIRS measurement of deoxyhemoglobin than with the fNIRS measurement of oxyhemoglobin or total hemoglobin. This result was predicted from the theoretical basis of the BOLD response (as the BOLD response is based on changes in the concentration of HbR) and from previous publications (Toronov et al., 2001).

Averaged hemodynamic response function for simultaneously recorded fNIRS and BOLD responses during finger tapping. Maximum change was normalized to unity, and the HbR response was inverted. The error bars on plot (A) represent the standard error of each (more...)

Using a more complex cognitive paradigm, target categorization, Bunce et al. (2006) replicated the fMRI protocol of McCarthy et al. (1997) using fNIRS. The results location and time course of McCarthy et al. are displayed in Figure 2-4 , Row A. The fNIRS results were quite similar to the fMRI results reported by McCarthy et al. (1997).

Replication of the fMRI protocol of McCarthy et al. (1997) using fNIRS. SOURCE: (A) McCarthy et al. (1997). ©1997 by the American Physiological Society; reprinted with permission. (B) (Image) Digital Anatomist images are from the Neuro-anatomy (more...)

fNIRS has also been reported to measure changes in the optical properties of the cell membranes themselves that occur when a neuron fires (Gratton et al., 1995), referred to as an event-related optical signal (EROS). Although the signal-to-noise ratio is low in current technological incarnations, this latter measure is particularly interesting as it represents the holy grail of neuroimaging; high spatial resolution coupled to high temporal resolution. (Please see Appendix D for a more thorough discussion of fNIRS technology.)

Of importance in military applications, fNIRS is safe, noninvasive, and highly portable, even wireless. Subjects are able to sit upright, work on computers or perform other tasks, even walk on treadmills (Izzetoglu et al., 2004). With near-zero run-time costs, fNIRS is also inexpensive. Although current systems cost between $25,000 and $300,000, they are still largely bench-made. Continuous-wave systems could be manufactured for a few thousand dollars, and probably for less in volume, especially as the cost of manufacturing lasers and light-emitting diodes continues to fall (see Appendix D for further information on system types). Extant systems operate from a laptop computer and a 2 × 4 × 6 inch control box. Technological advances currently under development include having the entire system on a digital signal processing chip operating from a laptop computer and linked to a wireless sensor. These properties of fNIRS make neuroimaging possible where other neuroimaging technologies are impractical or impossible. Preliminary studies have been conducted with fNIRS in the backpacks of warfighters walking through virtual reality programs. Other advances currently under investigation include closed-loop human brain–computer interfaces and implantable optodes. Implantable optodes could allow realizing the holy grail of neuroimaging, the direct, noninvasive measurement of neuronal activity with millisecond-level time resolution and superior spatial resolution.

fNIRS measures relative changes in HbO and HbR, and total blood flow can be calculated from the differential equation. There are no approved clinical neuroimaging uses for this technique, but there are several advantages for experi mental use. fNIRS systems are not as susceptible to movement artifact as fMRI, and algorithms are being refined for the removal of such artifacts (Izzetoglu et al., 2004). fNIRS depends on measurements of energy outside the visible spectrum. fNIRS was first used during World War I to monitor the blood oxygenation of bomber crews, a critical measurement before pressurized cabins were introduced in B-29s. Although fNIRS research has been ongoing since the late 1930s, the recent breakthroughs in both fNIRS and fMRI research have renewed interest in this technology. The capacity to translate findings from fMRI into fieldable, user-friendly, wearable devices is of significant interest. For instance, fNIRS has been shown to have promise in the detection of deception (Bunce et al., 2005), being both affordable and fieldable. The potential experimental uses of this technology are very exciting and include ecologically valid brain–computer interfaces; neurofeedback for guided facilitation of neural plasticity; and wearable neural monitors. Research groups in Japan (Haida et al., 2000), Ireland (Coyle et al., 2007), and the United States are working on brain–computer interfaces that allow locked-in patients (patients with no motor control, such as amyotrophic lateral sclerosis (ALS) patients) to communicate. In addition, Singapore has asked researchers in the United States to develop a brain fingerprint to identify specific brain signatures using fNIRS. Some advantages of the technology are its moderate cost (between $25,000 and $300,000) (Duscheck and Schandry, 2003) and its temporal resolution, which is similar to although somewhat lower than that of fMRI (1 cm 3 ). 13 It is also portable, wireless, and completely noninvasive.

These attributes allow fNIRS to be used with children and with patients who may find confinement in an fMRI magnet very unpleasant. A number of sensor applications exist, including caps, tension straps, and medical-grade adhesives. fNIRS is quiet and comfortable and therefore amenable to sensitive protocols such as the induction of positive moods and to integration with other technologies, including EEG. Appendix D provides additional information on the cost of the technology. NIRS can theoretically be combined with EEG, transcranial sonography, and other functional neuroimaging sensors. Unlike fMRI, where the subject is confined to the bore of the magnet, NIRS movement artifacts can be limited by proper affixation of sensors to the scalp. The major limitation is that NIRS best measures the first 2 or 3 centimeters of cortex so that deep brain imaging, at least through an intact skull, is challenging. Ongoing work, however, suggests that soon they will be able to image up to 5 cm deep.

Monitoring Advanced Cognitive Processes via Neuroimaging

There is a large body of published research on the use of various neuroimaging modalities to investigate the neural circuitry associated with deception. Recent PET and fMRI studies have provided insights, with specific areas in the prefrontal cortices and amygdala being the most commonly implicated regions (Abe et al., 2006, 2007; Mohamed et al., 2006; Davatzikos et al., 2005; Kozel et al., 2004a,b, 2005; Langleben et al., 2002, 2005; Lee et al., 2002, 2005; Nuñez et al., 2005; Phan et al., 2005a; Ganis et al., 2003; Spence et al., 2001). Recent NIRs studies of deception have also implicated prefrontal brain regions in the neural circuitry associated with deception (Bunce et al., 2005). Another recently published study that correlated fMRI measurements with standard skin conductance measurements during a concealed information paradigm had interesting results (Gamer et al., 2007). There are other possible uses for fMRI and other neuroimaging technologies that would indirectly provide information about deception and that are far more likely to be successful in the near future. These indirect measures would not require any response from the subject but would provide passive information about the subject’s experience. fMRI can already be used to judge recognition of items on a trial-by-trial basis. For example, one can imagine showing a subject a series of pictures of other people or crime scenes, and using fMRI to detect those that are familiar to the subject. The fMRI data could then be compared to the subject’s own statements about familiarity; this would be an indirect measure of lying. Such trial-by-trial measures are already under active investigation in the fMRI field, and it is entirely possible that they could be enhanced to aid in detection of deception using targeted research funding.

Finding 2-5. Functional neuroimaging is progressing rapidly and is likely to produce important findings over the next two decades. For the intelligence community and the Department of Defense, two areas in which such progress could be of great interest are enhancing cognition and facilitating training. Additional research is still needed on states of emotion; motivation; psychopathology; language; imaging processing for measuring workload performance; and the differences between Western and non-Western cultures.

  • Abe, Nobuhito, Maki Suzuki, Etsuro Mori, Masatoshi Itoh, and Toshikatsu Fujii. 2007. Deceiving others: Distinct neural responses of the prefrontal cortex and amygdala in simple fabrication and deception with social interactions . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 19(2):287-295. [ PubMed : 17280517 ]
  • Abe, Nobuhito, Maki Suzuki, Takashi Tsukiura, Etsuro Mori, Keiichiro Yamaguchi, Masatoshi Itoh, and Toshikatsu Fujii. 2006. Dissociable roles of prefrontal and anterior cingulate cortices in deception . Cerebral Cortex 16(2):192-199. [ PubMed : 15858160 ]
  • Abraham, Michael H., Harpreet S. Chadha, and Robert C. Mitchell. 1994. Hydrogen bonding. 33. Factors that influence the distribution of solutes between blood and brain . Journal of Pharma ceutical Sciences 83(9):1257-1268. [ PubMed : 7830242 ]
  • Altoff, R.R., and Cohen, N.J. 1999. Eye-movement-based memory effect: Reprocessing effect in face perception . Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 25: 997-1010. [ PubMed : 10439505 ]
  • Beduneau, A., P. Saulnier, N. Anton, F. Hindre, C. Passirani, H. Rajerison, N. Noiret, and J.P. Benoit. 2006. Pegylated nanocapsules produced by an organic solvent-free method: Evaluation of their stealth properties . Pharmaceutical Research 23(9):2190-2199. [ PubMed : 16952009 ]
  • Beecher, H.K. 1955. The powerful placebo . Journal of the American Medical Association 159:1602-1606. [ PubMed : 13271123 ]
  • Bentivoglio, Marina, and Krister Kristensson. 2007. Neural–immune interactions in disorders of sleep-wakefulness organization . Trends in Neurosciences 30(12):645-652. [ PubMed : 17988749 ]
  • Berger, H. 1929. Uber das elektrenkephalogramm des menschen . Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 87(1):527-580.
  • Blascovich, J. 2000. Psychophysiological methods . In Handbook of Research Methods in Social and Personality Psychology , H.T. Reis, editor; and C.M. Judd, editor. , eds. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
  • Blascovich, J., and J. Tomaka. 1996. The biopsychosocial model of arousal regulation . In Advances in Experimental Social Psychology , M. Zanna, editor. , ed. New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Blascovich, J., and M. Seery. 2007. Visceral and somatic indexes of social psychological constructs: History, principles, propositions, and case studies . In Social Psychology: Handbook of Basic Principles , A.W. Kruglanski, editor; and E.T. Higgins, editor. , eds. New York, NY: Guilford.
  • Born, Jan, Tanja Lange, Werner Kern, Gerard P. McGregor, Ulrich Bickel, and Horst L. Fehm. 2002. Sniffing neuropeptides: A transnasal approach to the human brain . Nature Neuroscience 5(6):514-516. [ PubMed : 11992114 ]
  • Bosi, Susanna, Tatiana Da Ros, Sabrina Castellano, Elena Banfi, and Maurizio Prato. 2000. Antimycobacterial activity of ionic fullerene derivatives . Bioorganic and Medicinal Chemistry Letters 10(10):1043-1045. [ PubMed : 10843212 ]
  • Brazier, Mary A.B. 1949. The electrical fields at the surface of the head during sleep . Electroencepha lography and Clinical Neurophysiology 1(2; May):195-204. [ PubMed : 18144877 ]
  • Brightman, M.W., and T.S. Reese. 1969. Junctions between intimately apposed cell membranes in the vertebrate brain . Journal of Cell Biology 40(3):648-677. [ PMC free article : PMC2107650 ] [ PubMed : 5765759 ]
  • Bunce, Scott C., Ajit Devaraj, Meltem Izzetoglu, Banu Onaral, and Kambiz Pourrezaei. 2005. Detecting deception in the brain: A functional near-infrared spectroscopy study of neural correlates of intentional deception . Proceedings of SPIE 5769:24-32.
  • Bunce, Scott C., Meltem Izzetoglu, Kurtulus Izzetoglu, Banu Onaral, and Kambiz Pourrezaei. 2006. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy: An emerging neuroimaging modality . IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Magazine 25(4):54-62. [ PubMed : 16898659 ]
  • Cabeza, R., and L. Nyberg. 2000. Imaging cognition II: An empirical review of 275 PET and fMRI studies . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 12(1):1-47. [ PubMed : 10769304 ]
  • Cacioppo, J.T., and L.G. Tassinary. 1990. a. Principles of Psychophysiology: Physical, Social, and Inferential Elements . New York: Cambridge University Press.
  • Cacioppo, J.T., and L.G. Tassinary. 1990. b. Inferring psychological significance from physiological signals . American Psychologist 45(1):16-28. [ PubMed : 2297166 ]
  • Cacioppo, J.T., R.E. Petty, M.E. Losch, and H.S. Kim. 1986. Electromyographic activity over facial muscle regions can differentiate the valence and intensity of affective reactions . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 50(2):260-268. [ PubMed : 3701577 ]
  • Cade, John F. 1949. Lithium salts in the treatment of psychotic excitement . Medical Journal of Australia 2:349-352. [ PubMed : 18142718 ]
  • Cao, Cong, Richard P. Suttmeier, and Denis Fred Simon. 2006. China’s 15-year science and technology plan . Physics Today 59(12):38-43.
  • Carter, C. Sue. 1998. Neuroendocrine perspectives on social attachment and love . Psychoneuro endocrinology 23(8):779-818. [ PubMed : 9924738 ]
  • Carter, C. Sue, Margaret Altemus, and George P. Chrousos. 2001. Neuroendocrine and emotional changes in the post-partum period . Progress in Brain Research 133:241-249. [ PubMed : 11589134 ]
  • Chen, Robert J., Yuegang Zhang, Dunwei Wang, and Hongjie Dai. 2001. Noncovalent sidewall functionalization of single-walled carbon nanotubes for protein immobilization . Journal of the American Chemical Society 123(16):3838-3839. [ PubMed : 11457124 ]
  • Coyle, D., T.M. McGinnity, and G. Prasad. 2007. Identifying local ultrametricity of EEG time series for feature extraction in a brain-computer interface . Pp. 701-704 in Proceedings of the 29th Annual International Conference of the IEEE Engineering in Medicine and Biology Society . [ PubMed : 18002053 ]
  • Davatzikos, C., K. Ruparel, Y. Fan, D.G. Shen, M. Acharyya, J.W. Loughead, R.C. Gur, and D.D. Langleben. 2005. Classifying spatial patterns of brain activity with machine learning methods: Application to lie detection . NeuroImage 28(3):663-668. [ PubMed : 16169252 ]
  • Davis, P.A. 1939. Effects of acoustic stimuli on the waking human brain . Journal of Neurophysiology 2:494-499.
  • Dews, P.B., and G.R. Wenger. 1977. Rate-dependency of the behavioral effects of amphetamine . In Advances in Behavioral Pharmacology , Vol. 1,T. Thompson, editor; and P.B. Dews, editor. , eds. New York, NY: Academic Press.
  • Dickerson, B.C. 2007. Advances in functional magnetic resonance imaging: technology and clinical applications . Neurotherapeutics 4(3):360-70. [ PMC free article : PMC7479713 ] [ PubMed : 17599702 ]
  • Dickerson, Sally S. and Margaret E. Kemeny. 2004. Acute stressors and cortisol responses: A theoretical integration and synthesis of laboratory research . Psychological Bulletin 130(3):355-391. [ PubMed : 15122924 ]
  • Dunning, Mark D., Andras Lakatos, Louiza Loizou, Mikko Kettunen, Charles French-Constant, Kevin M. Brindle, and Robin J.M. Franklin. 2004. Superparamagnetic iron oxide-labeled Schwann cells and olfactory ensheathing cells can be traced in vivo by magnetic resonance imaging and retain functional properties after transplantation into the CNS . Journal of Neuroscience 24(44):9799-9810. [ PMC free article : PMC6730225 ] [ PubMed : 15525765 ]
  • Duscheck, Stephen, and Rainer Schandry. 2003. Functional transcranial Doppler sonography as a tool in psychophysiological research . Psychophysiology 40(3):436-54. [ PubMed : 12946117 ]
  • Ekman, Paul, and Maureen O’Sullivan. 2006. From flawed self-assessment to blatant whoppers: The utility of voluntary and involuntary behavior in detecting deception . Behavioral Sciences and the Law 24(5):673-686. [ PubMed : 17016820 ]
  • El Kaliouby, R., and P. Robinson. 2005. The Emotional Hearing Aid: An assistive tool for children with Asperger syndrome . Universal Access in the Information Society 4(2):121-134.
  • Ellis-Behnke, R.G., L.A. Teather, G.E. Schneider, and K.F. So. 2007. Using nanotechnology to design potential therapies for CNS regeneration . Current Pharmaceutical Design 13(24):2519-2528. [ PubMed : 17692020 ]
  • Emerich, Dwaine F., and Christopher G. Thanos. 2006. The pinpoint promise of nanoparticle-based drug delivery and molecular diagnosis . Biomolecular Engineering 23(4):171-184. [ PubMed : 16843058 ]
  • Estes, William. 2002. Traps in the route to models of memory and decision . Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 9(1):3-25. [ PubMed : 12026952 ]
  • Fender, D.H. 1987. Source localization of brain electrical activity . Pp. 355-403 in Methods of Analy sis of Brain Electrical and Magnetic Signals , A.S. Gevins, editor; and A. Remond, editor. , eds. Amsterdam: Elsevier.
  • Feynman, Richard P. 1959. “There is plenty of room at the bottom,” Lecture given at Annual Meeting of the American Physical Society, December 29, 1959, at California Institute of Technology, Pasadena, CA.
  • Floresco, Stan B., Mark A. Geyer, Lisa H. Gold, and Anthony A. Grace. 2005. Developing predictive animal models and establishing a preclinical trials network for assessing treatment effects on cognition in schizophrenia . Schizophrenia Bulletin 31(4):888-894. [ PubMed : 16079387 ]
  • Foer, Joshua. 2007. Remember this . National Geographic 212(5):32.
  • Fox, Daniel J., David F. Tharp, and Lydia C. Fox. 2005. Neurofeedback: An alternative and efficacious treatment for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder . Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback 30(4):365-373. [ PubMed : 16385424 ]
  • Frank, Mark G., and Paul Ekman. 2004. Appearing truthful generalizes across different deception situations . Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 86(3):486-495. [ PubMed : 15008651 ]
  • Freitas, Robert A., Jr. 1999. Nanomedicine, Vol. 1: Basic Capabilities . Austin, TX: Landes Bioscience.
  • Gamer, M., T. Bauermann, P. Stoeter, and G. Vossel. 2007. Covariations among fMRI , skin conductance , and behavioral data during processing of concealed information . Human Brain Mapping 28(12):1287-1301. [ PMC free article : PMC6871443 ] [ PubMed : 17290371 ]
  • Ganis, G., S.M. Kosslyn, S. Stose, W.L. Thompson, and D.A. Yurgelun-Todd. 2003. Neural correlates of different types of deception: An fMRI investigation . Cerebral Cortex 13(8):830-836. [ PubMed : 12853369 ]
  • Gao, Xiaoling, Jun Chen, Weixing Tao, Jianhua Zhu, Qizhi Zhang, Hongzhuan Chen, and Xinguo Jiang. 2007. a. UEA I-bearing nanoparticles for brain delivery following intranasal administration . International Journal of Pharmaceutics 340(1-2):207-215. [ PubMed : 17499948 ]
  • Gao, Xiaoling, Bingxian Wu, Qizhi Zhang, Jun Chen, Jianhua Zhu, Weiwei Zhang, Zhengxin Rong, Hongzhuan Chen, and Xinguo Jianga. 2007. b. Brain delivery of vasoactive intestinal peptide enhanced with the nanoparticles conjugated with wheat germ agglutinin following intranasal administration . Journal of Controlled Release 121(3):156-167. [ PubMed : 17628165 ]
  • Golden, Pamela L., and Gary M. Pollack. 2003. Blood-brain barrier efflux transport . Journal of Pharmaceutical Sciences 92(9):1739-1753. [ PubMed : 12949994 ]
  • Goldstein, Alex S., John K. Amory, Stephanie M. Martin, Chris Vernon, Alvin Matsumoto, and Paul Yager. 2001. Testosterone delivery using glutamide-based complex high axial ratio microstructures . Bioorganic and Medicinal Chemistry Letters 9(11):2819-2825. [ PubMed : 11597462 ]
  • Gourley, Paul L. 2005. Brief overview of BioMicroNano technologies . Biotechnology Progress 21(1):2-10. [ PubMed : 15903234 ]
  • Grayson, Amy C.R., Insung S. Choi, Betty M. Tyler, Paul P. Wang, Henry Brem, Michael J. Cima, and Robert Langer. 2003. Multi-pulse drug delivery from a resorbable polymeric microchip device . Nature Materials 2(11):767-772. [ PubMed : 14619935 ]
  • Green, D.M., and J.A. Swets. 1966. Signal Detection Theory and Psychophysics . New York, NY: Wiley.
  • Guastella, Adam J., Phillip B. Mitchell, and Mark R. Dadds. 2008. Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye region of human faces . Biological Psychiatry 63(1):3-5. [ PubMed : 17888410 ]
  • Guertin,W.H., and P.L. Wilhelm. 1954. A statistical analysis of the electrodermal response employed in lie detection . Journal of General Psychology 51:153-160.
  • Gururangan, S., and H. Friedman. 2002. Innovations in design and delivery of chemotherapy for brain tumors . Neuroimaging Clinics of North America 12(4):583-597. [ PubMed : 12687913 ]
  • Hagan, J.J., and D.N. Jones. 2005. Predicting drug efficacy for cognitive deficits in schizophrenia . Schizophrenia Bulletin 31(4):830-853. [ PubMed : 16177277 ]
  • Haida, M., Y. Shinohara, Y. Ito, T. Yamamoto, F. Kawaguchi, and H. Koizumi. 2000. Brain function of an ALS patient in complete locked-in state by using optical topography . Pp. 95–97 in The Frontier of Mind-Brain Science and Its Practical Applications (II) , H. Koizumi, editor. , ed. Tokyo: Hitachi Ltd.
  • Hamaguchi, T., Y. Matsumura, M. Suzuki, K. Shimizu, R. Goda, I. Nakamura, I. Nakatomi, M. Yokoyama, K. Kataoka, and T. Kakizoe. 2005. NK105, a paclitaxel-incorporating micellar nanoparticle formulation, can extend in vivo antitumour activity and reduce the neurotoxicity of paclitaxel . British Journal of Cancer 92(7):1240-1246. [ PMC free article : PMC2361981 ] [ PubMed : 15785749 ]
  • Hamilton, Alice. 1915. Lead poisoning in the manufacture of storage batteries . P. 27 in Bulletin of the United States Bureau of Labor Statistics , No. 165. Washington, D.C.: Government Printing Office.
  • Hammoud, D.A., J.M. Hoffman, and M.G. Pomper. 2007. Molecular neuroimaging: From conventional to emerging techniques . Radiology 245(1):21-42. [ PubMed : 17885179 ]
  • Heinrichs, Markus, Inga Neumann, and Ulrike Ehlert. 2002. Lactation and stress: Protective effects of breast-feeding in humans . Stress 5(3):195-203. [ PubMed : 12186682 ]
  • Heinrichs, Markus, Thomas Baumgartner, Clemens Kirschbaum, and Ulrike Ehlert. 2003. Social support and oxytocin interact to suppress cortisol and subjective responses to psychosocial stress . Biological Psychiatry 54(12):1389-1398. [ PubMed : 14675803 ]
  • Heinrichs, Markus, Gunther Meinlschmidt, Werner Wippich, Ulrike Ehlert, and Dirk H. Hellhammer. 2004. Selective amnesic effects of oxytocin on human memory . Physiology and Behavior 83(1):31-38. [ PubMed : 15501488 ]
  • Hillyer, Julián F., and Ralph M. Albrecht. 2001. Gastrointestinal persorption and tissue distribution of differently sized colloidal gold nanoparticles . Journal of Pharmaceutical Sciences 90(12):1927-1936. [ PubMed : 11745751 ]
  • Hirsch, Lee R., Naomi J. Halas, and Jennifer L. West. 2005. Whole-blood immunoassay facilitated by gold nanoshell-conjugate antibodies . Methods in Molecular Biology 303 (April):101-111. [ PubMed : 15923678 ]
  • Huber, Daniel, Pierre Veinante, and Ron Stoop. 2005. Vasopressin and oxytocin excite distinct neuronal populations in the central amygdala . Science 308(5719):245-248. [ PubMed : 15821089 ]
  • Huppert, T.J., R.D. Hoge, S.G. Diamond, M.A. Franceschini, and D.A. Boas. 2006. A temporal comparison of BOLD, ASL, and NIRS hemodynamic responses to motor stimuli in adult humans . NeuroImage 29(2):368-382. [ PMC free article : PMC2692693 ] [ PubMed : 16303317 ]
  • Hussain, N., V. Jaitley, and A.T. Florence. 2001. Recent advances in the understanding of uptake of microparticulates across the gastrointestinal lymphatics . Advanced Drug Delivery Reviews 50(1):107-142. [ PubMed : 11489336 ]
  • Iacono, W.G. 2000. The detection of deception . Handbook of Psychophysiology , 2nd Ed., J.T. Cacioppo, editor; , L.G. Tassinary, editor; , and G.G. Berntson, editor. , eds. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.
  • Illum, Lisbeth. 2000. Transport of drugs from the nasal cavity to the central nervous system . European Journal of Pharmaceutical Sciences 11(1):1-18. [ PubMed : 10913748 ]
  • Insel, Thomas R., and Larry J. Young. 2001. The neurobiology of attachment . Nature Reviews Neuro science 2(2):129-136. [ PubMed : 11252992 ]
  • International Labour Office. 1934. Accumulators (storage batteries) . P. 29 in Occupation and Health: An Encyclopaedia of Hygiene, Pathology and Social Welfare , Vol. 1. Geneva, Switzerland: International Labour Office.
  • Izzetoglu, K., S. Bunce, B. Onaral, K. Pourrezaei, and B. Chance. 2004. Functional optical brain imaging using NIR during cognitive tasks . International Journal of Human-Computer Interaction 17(2):211-227.
  • Jain, K.K. 2007. Nanobiotechnology-based drug delivery to the central nervous system . Neuro degenerative Diseases 4(4):287-291. [ PubMed : 17627131 ]
  • James, William. 1890. Principles of Psychology . Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
  • Jarvik, Murray. 1970. Drugs used in the treatment of psychiatric disorders . The Pharmacological Basis of Therapeutics , L. Goodman, editor; and H. Gilman, editor. , eds. New York, NY: Macmillian.
  • Kleiman, M. 1992. Against Excess: Drug Policy for Results . New York, NY: Basic Books.
  • Koo, Yong-Eun E., Ramachandra G. Reddy, Mahaveer Bhojani, Randy Schneider, Martin A. Philbert, Alnawaz Rehemtulla, Brian D. Ross, and Raoul Kopelman. 2006. Brain cancer diagnosis and therapy with nanoplatforms . Advanced Drug Delivery Reviews 58(14):1556-1577. [ PubMed : 17107738 ]
  • Kosfeld, Michael, Markus Heinrichs, Paul J. Zak, Urs Fischbacher, and Ernst Fehr. 2005. Oxytocin increases trust in humans . Nature 435(7042):673-676. [ PubMed : 15931222 ]
  • Koushik, K., D.S. Dhanda, N.P.S. Cheruvu, and U.B. Kompella. 2004. Pulmonary delivery of deslorelin: Large-porous PLGA particles and HPbetaCD complexes . Pharmaceutical Research 21(7):1119-1126. [ PubMed : 15290850 ]
  • Kozak, Rouba, John P. Bruno, and Martin Sarter. 2006. Augmented prefrontal acetylcholine release during challenged attentional performance . Cerebral Cortex 16(1):9-17. [ PubMed : 15788700 ]
  • Kozel, F.A., T.M. Padgett, and M.S. George. 2004. a. A replication study of the neural correlates of deception . Behavioral Neuroscience 118(4):852-856. [ PubMed : 15301611 ]
  • Kozel, F.A., Letty J. Revell, Jeffrey P. Lorberbaum, Ananda Shastri, Jon D. Elhai, Michael David Horner, Adam Smith, Ziad Nahas, Daryl E. Bohning, and Mark S. George. 2004. b. A pilot study of functional magnetic resonance imaging brain correlates of deception in healthy young men . Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences 16(3):295-305. [ PubMed : 15377736 ]
  • Kozel, F.A., K.A. Johnson, Q. Mu, E.L. Grenesko, S.J. Laken, and M.S. George. 2005. Detecting deception using functional magnetic resonance imaging . Biological Psychiatry 58(8):605-613. [ PubMed : 16185668 ]
  • Ladd, M.E. 2007. High-field-strength magnetic resonance: Potential and limits . Topics in Magnetic Resonance Imaging 18(2):139-152. [ PubMed : 17621228 ]
  • Landgraf, Rainer, and Inga D. Neumann. 2004. Vasopressin and oxytocin release within the brain: A dynamic concept of multiple and variable modes of neuropeptide communication . Frontiers in Neuroendocrinology 25(3-4):150-176. [ PubMed : 15589267 ]
  • Langleben, D.D., L. Schroeder, J.A. Maldjian, R.C. Gur, S. McDonald, J.D. Raglanda, C.P. O’Brien, and A.R. Childress. 2002. Brain activity during simulated deception: An event-related functional magnetic resonance study . Neuroimage 15(3):727-732. [ PubMed : 11848716 ]
  • Langleben, D.D., James W. Loughead, Warren B. Bilker, Kosha Rupare, Anna Rose Childress, Samantha I. Busch, and Ruben C. Gur. 2005. Telling truth from lie in individual subjects with fast event-related fMRI . Human Brain Mapping 26(4):262-272. [ PMC free article : PMC6871667 ] [ PubMed : 16161128 ]
  • Langston, J. William. 1995. The Case of the Frozen Addict . New York, NY: Pantheon Press.
  • Lasagna L., F. Mosteller, J.M. von Felsinger, and H.K. Beecher. 1954. A study of the placebo response . American Journal of Medicine 16:770-779. [ PubMed : 13158365 ]
  • Lavie, N. 2005. Distracted and confused? Selective attention under load . Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9(2):75-82. [ PubMed : 15668100 ]
  • Lee, Tatia M.C., Ho-Ling Liu, Li-Hai Tan, Chetwyn C.H. Chan, Srikanth Mahankali, Ching-Mei Feng, Jinwen Hou, Peter T. Fox, and Jia-Hong Gao. 2002. Lie detection by functional magnetic resonance imaging . Human Brain Mapping 15(3):157-164. [ PMC free article : PMC6872015 ] [ PubMed : 11835606 ]
  • Lee, Tatia M.C., Wen-hua Zhou, Xiao-jing Luo, Kenneth S.L. Yuen, Xin-zhong Ruan, and Xu-chu Weng. 2005. Neural activity associated with cognitive regulation in heroin users: A fMRI study . Neuroscience Letters 382(3):211-216. [ PubMed : 15925092 ]
  • Loo, C., L. Hirsch, M.H. Lee, E. Chang, J. West, N. Halas, and R. Drezek. 2005. Gold nanoshell bioconjugates for molecular imaging in living cells . Optics Letters 30(9):1012-1014. [ PubMed : 15906987 ]
  • Maloney, John M., Scott A. Uhland, Benjamin F. Polito, Norman F. Sheppard, Christina M. Pelta, and John T. Santini. 2005. Electrothermally activated microchips for implantable drug delivery and biosensing . Journal of Controlled Release 109(1-3):244-255. [ PubMed : 16278032 ]
  • Manji, H.K., J.M. Bebchuk, G.J. Moore, D. Glitz, K. Hasanat, G. Chen. 1999. Modulation of CNS signal transduction pathways and gene expression by mood stabilizing agents: Therapeutic implications . Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 60(S2):27-39. [ PubMed : 10073385 ]
  • Marchak, Frank M. 2006. Eye movement-based assessment of concealed knowledge . Journal of Credibility Assessment and Witness Psychology 7(2):148-163.
  • Martin, Charles R., and Punit Kohli. 2003. The emerging field of nanotube biotechnology . Nature Reviews Drug Discovery 2(1):29-37. [ PubMed : 12509757 ]
  • McCarthy, Gregory, Marie Luby, John Gore, and Patricia Goldman-Rakic. 1997. Infrequent events transiently activate human prefrontal and parietal cortex as measured by functional MRI . Journal of Neurophysiology 77(3):1630-1634. [ PubMed : 9084626 ]
  • Merkus, Frans W.H.M., and Mascha P. van den Berg. 2007. Can nasal drug delivery bypass the blood-brain barrier?: Questioning the direct transport theory . Drugs in R & D 8(3):133-144. [ PubMed : 17472409 ]
  • Michel, Christopher M., Micah M. Murray, Göran Lantz, Sara Gonzalez, Laurent Spinelli, and Rolando Grave de Peralta. 2004. EEG source imaging . Clinical Neurophysiology 115:2195-2222. [ PubMed : 15351361 ]
  • Miller, M.B., A. Kingstone, and M.S. Gazzaniga. 2002. a. Hemispheric encoding asymmetries are more apparent than real . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(5):702-708. [ PubMed : 12167255 ]
  • Miller, M.B., J. Van Horn, G.L. Wolford, T.C. Handy, M. Valsangkar-Smyth, S. Inati, S. Grafton, and M.S. Gazzaniga. 2002. b. Extensive individual differences in brain activations during episodic retrieval are reliable over time . Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 14(8):1200-1214. [ PubMed : 12495526 ]
  • Miyata, N., and T. Yamakoshi. 1997. Fullerenes: Recent advances in the chemistry and physics of fullerenes and related materials . P. 345 in Proceedings of the Symposium on Recent Advances in the Chemistry and Physics of Fullerenes and Related Materials , Vol. 5,R.S. Ruoff, editor; and K.M. Kadish, editor. , eds. Pennington, NJ: Electrochemical Society.
  • Mohamed, Feroze B., Scott H. Faro, Nathan J. Gordon, Steven M. Platek, Harris Ahmad, and J. Michael Williams. 2006. Brain mapping of deception and truth telling about an ecologically valid situation: Functional MR imaging and polygraph investigation—initial experience . Radiology 238(2):679-688. [ PubMed : 16436822 ]
  • Moore, Gregory J. 1998. Proton magnetic resonance spectroscopy in pediatric neuroradiology . Pediatric Radiology 28(11):805-814. [ PubMed : 9799306 ]
  • Moore, Gregory J., J.M. Bebchuk, K. Hasanat, M.W. Faulk, N. Seraji-Bozorgzad, I.B. Wilds, C.L. Arfken, G. Chen, and H.K. Manji. 2000. Lithium increases N-acetyl-aspartate in the human brain: In vivo evidence in support of bcl-2’s neurotrophic effects? Biological Psychiatry 48(1):1-8. [ PubMed : 10913502 ]
  • Musto, D. 1987. The American Disease: Origins of Narcotic Control . 2nd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
  • Nakada, T. 2007. Clinical application of high and ultra high-field MRI . Brain and Development 29(6):325-335. [ PubMed : 17113259 ]
  • Nishino, S. 2007. a. Narcolepsy: Pathophysiology and pharmacology . The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry 68(Suppl. 13):9-15. [ PubMed : 18078360 ]
  • Nishino, S. 2007. b. The hypocretin/orexin receptor: Therapeutic prospective in sleep disorders . Expert Opinion on Investigational Drugs 16(11):1785-1797. [ PubMed : 17970638 ]
  • Nitsche, Michael A., Christian Lampe, Andrea Antal, David Liebetanz, Nicolas Lang, Frithjof Tergau, and Walter Paulus. 2006. Dopaminergic modulation of long-lasting direct current-induced cortical excitability changes in the human motor cortex . European Journal of Neuroscience 23(6):1651-1657. [ PubMed : 16553629 ]
  • NRC (National Research Council). 2003. The Polygraph and Lie Detection . Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. Available from http://www ​.nap.edu/catalog ​.php?record_id=10420 .
  • Nuñez, Jennifer Maria, B.J. Casey, Tobias Egner, Todd Hare, and Joy Hirsch. 2005. Intentional false responding shares neural substrates with response conflict and cognitive control . NeuroImage 25(1):267-277. [ PubMed : 15734361 ]
  • Olshansky, Brian. 2007. Placebo and nocebo in cardiovascular health. Implications for healthcare, research, and the doctor-patient relationship . Journal of the American College of Cardiology 49:415-421. Available from http://content ​.onlinejacc ​.org/cgi/reprint/49/4/415 . Last accessed June 18, 2008. [ PubMed : 17258086 ]
  • OTA (Office of Technology Assessment of the U.S. Congress). 1983. Scientific Validity of Polygraph Testing: A Research Review and Evaluation . OTA-TM-H-15. Washington, DC: U.S. Congress.
  • OTA. 1990. The Use of Integrity Tests for Pre-employment Screening . OTA-SET-442. Washington, DC: U.S. Congress.
  • Panyam, Jayanth, and Vinod Labhasetwar. 2003. Biodegradable nanoparticles for drug and gene delivery to cells and tissue . Advanced Drug Delivery Reviews 55(3):329-347. [ PubMed : 12628320 ]
  • Pardridge, William M. 1999. Blood-brain barrier biology and methodology . Journal of NeuroVirology 5(6):556-569. [ PubMed : 10602397 ]
  • Pardridge, William M. 2001. BBB-genomics: Creating new openings for brain-drug targeting . Drug Discovery Today 6(8):381-383. [ PubMed : 11301273 ]
  • Pedersen, Cort A. 1997. Oxytocin control of maternal behavior: Regulation by sex steroids and offspring stimuli . Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 807:126-145. [ PubMed : 9071347 ]
  • Peters, David, Richard Genik, and Christopher Green. 2008. Neuroimaging in defense policy . Pp. 1-42 in Biotechnology and the Future of America’s Military: The “New” Biological Warfare . Washington, DC: National Defense University Press.
  • Petrik, Vladimir, Vinothini Apok, Juliet Britton, B. Anthony Bell, and Marios Papadopoulos. 2006. Godfrey Hounsfield and the dawn of computed tomography . Neurosurgery 58(4):780-787. [ PubMed : 16575342 ]
  • Phan, K. Luan, Daniel A. Fitzgerald, Kunxiu Gao, Gregory J. Moore, Manuel E. Tancer, and Stefan Posse. 2004. Real-time fMRI of cortico-limbic brain activity during emotional processing . NeuroReport 15(3):527-532. [ PubMed : 15094517 ]
  • Phan, K. Luan, Alvaro Magalhaes, Timothy J. Ziemlewicz, Daniel A. Fitzgerald, Christopher Green, and Wilbur Smith. 2005. a. Neural correlates of telling lies: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study at 4 tesla . Academic Radiology 12(2):164-172. [ PubMed : 15721593 ]
  • Phan, K. Luan, Daniel A. Fitzgerald, Pradeep J. Nathan, Gregory J. Moore, Thomas W. Uhde, and Manuel E. Tancer. 2005. b. Neural substrates for voluntary suppression of negative affect: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study . Biological Psychiatry 57(3):210-219. [ PubMed : 15691521 ]
  • Phan, K. Luan, Daniel A. Fitzgerald, Bernadette M. Cortese, Navid Seraji-Bozorgzad, Manuel E Tancer, and Gregory J. Moore. 2005. c. Anterior cingulate neurochemistry in social anxiety disorder: H-MRS at 4 tesla . NeuroReport 16(2):183-186. [ PubMed : 15671874 ]
  • Phelps, Michael E., Edward J. Hoffman, Nizar A. Mullani, and Michel M. Ter-Pogossian. 1975. Application of annihilation coincidence detection to transaxial reconstruction tomography . Journal of Nuclear Medicin e 16(3):210-224. [ PubMed : 1113170 ]
  • Plonsey, R. 1963. Reciprocity applied to volume conductors and the ECG . IEEE Transactions on Bio-Medical Engineering 10(Jan.):9-12. [ PubMed : 14059530 ]
  • Poo, Mu-ming, and Aike Guo. 2007. Some recent advances in basic neuroscience research in China . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society 362:10. [ PMC free article : PMC2435572 ] [ PubMed : 17329228 ]
  • Porges, Stephen W. 2006. The polygraph: One machine, two world views . Journal of Credibility Assessment and Witness Psychology 7:47-73.
  • Posse, Stefan, Daniel A. Fitzgerald, Kunxiu Gao, Ute Habel, David Rosenberg, Gregory J. Moore, and Frank Schneider. 2003. Real-time fMRI of temporolimbic regions detects amygdala activation during single-trial self-induced sadness . NeuroImage 18(3):760-768. [ PubMed : 12667853 ]
  • Prescott, James H., Sara Lipka, Samuel Baldwin, Norman F. Sheppard, John M. Maloney, Jonathan Coppeta, Barry Yomtov, Mark A. Staples, and John T. Santini. 2006. Chronic, programmed polypeptide delivery from an implanted, multireservoir microchip device . Nature Biotechnology 24(4):437-438. [ PubMed : 16531991 ]
  • Raichle, Marcus E. 2006. Neuroscience: The brain’s dark energy . Science 314(5830):1249-1250. [ PubMed : 17124311 ]
  • Reese, T.S., and Morris J. Karnovsky. 1967. Fine structural localization of a blood-brain barrier to exogenous peroxidase . Journal of Cell Biology 34(1):207-217. [ PMC free article : PMC2107213 ] [ PubMed : 6033532 ]
  • Robbins, T.W. 1998. Homology in behavioural pharmacology: An approach to animal models of human cognition . Behavioural Pharmacology 9:509-519. [ PubMed : 9862075 ]
  • Robbins, T.W. 2005. Chemistry of the mind: Neurochemical modulation of prefrontal cortical function . Journal of Comparative Neurology 493(1):140-146. [ PubMed : 16254988 ]
  • Rovner, Sophie L. 2007. Hold that thought . Chemical and Engineering News 85(36):13-20.
  • Sahankian, Barbara, and Sharon Morein-Zamir. 2007. Professor’s little helper . Nature 450:1157-1159 and accompanying discussion. Available from http://www ​.nature.com ​/nature/journal/v450 ​/n7173/full/4501157a.html . Last accessed 18 June 2008. [ PubMed : 18097378 ]
  • Santini, John T., Jr., Michael J. Cima, and Robert Langer. 1999. A controlled-release microchip . Nature 397(6717):335-338. [ PubMed : 9988626 ]
  • Sarter, M. 2006. Preclinical research into cognition enhancers . Trends in Pharmacological Sciences 27(11):602-608. [ PubMed : 16997388 ]
  • Sarter, M., and J Bruno. 1994. Cognitive functions of cortical ACh: Lessons from studies on trans-synaptic modulation of activated efflux . Trends in Neurosciences 17(6):217-221. [ PubMed : 7521080 ]
  • Saul, Stephanie. 2007. Sleep drugs found only mildly effective, but wildly popular . The New York Times , October 23, 2007.
  • Shapiro, D., and A. Crider. 1969. Psychophysiological approaches in social psychology . In Handbook of Social Psychology , Vol. III . (2nd Edition), G. Lindzey, editor; and E. Aronson, editor. , eds. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley.
  • Shaw, J.C., and M. Roth. 1955. Potential distribution analysis. I. A new technique for the analysis of electrophysiological phenomena . Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology 7(2; May):273-284. [ PubMed : 13241387 ]
  • Silva, Gabriel A. 2006. Neuroscience nanotechnology: Progress, opportunities and challenges . Nature Reviews Neuroscience 7(1):65-74. [ PubMed : 16371951 ]
  • Silva, Gabriel A. 2007. Nanotechnology approaches for drug and small molecule delivery across the blood brain barrier . Surgical Neurology 67(2):113-116. [ PubMed : 17254859 ]
  • Society for Neuroscience. 2007. Annual Meeting Statistics . Society for Neuroscience; 2007. Available from http://www ​.sfn.org/index ​.cfm?pagename=annualMeeting ​_statistics&section ​=annualMeeting . Last accessed June 18, 2008.
  • Spence, S.A., T.F. Farrow, A.E. Herford, I.D. Wilkinson, Y. Zheng, and, P.W. Woodruff. 2001. Behavioural and functional anatomical correlates of deception in humans . NeuroReport 12(13):2849-2853. [ PubMed : 11588589 ]
  • Spence, S.A., M.D. Hunter, T.F. Farrow, R.D. Green, D.H. Leung, C.J. Hughes, and V. Ganesan. 2004. A cognitive neurobiological account of deception: Evidence from functional neuroimaging . Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, Series B, Biological Sciences 359(1451):1755-1762. [ PMC free article : PMC1693447 ] [ PubMed : 15590616 ]
  • Strand, Fleur L. 1999. Neuropeptides: Regulators of Physiological Processes . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Suri, Sarabjeet Singh, Hicham Fenniri, and Baljit Singh. 2007. Nanotechnology-based drug delivery systems . Journal of Occupational Medicine and Toxicology 2:16. [ PMC free article : PMC2222591 ] [ PubMed : 18053152 ]
  • Swazey, Judith P. 1974. Chlorpromazine in Psychiatry: A Study of Therapeutic Innovation . Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
  • Tabata, Yasuhiko, Yoshiyuki Murakami, and Yoshito Ikada. 1997. a. Photodynamic effect of polyethylene glycol-modified fullerene on tumor . Japanese Journal of Cancer Research 88(11):1108-1116. [ PMC free article : PMC5921311 ] [ PubMed : 9439687 ]
  • Tabata, Yasuhiko, Yoshiyuki Murakami, and Yoshito Ikada. 1997. b. Antitumor effect of poly(ethylene glycol)-modified fullerene . Fullerenes, Nanotubes and Carbon Nanostructures 5(5):989-1007.
  • Taleb, Nassim Nicholas. 2007. a. The Black Swan: The Impact of the Highly Improbable . New York, NY: Random House.
  • Teixido, Meritxell, and Ernest Giralt. 2008. The role of peptides in blood-brain barrier nanotechnology . Journal of Peptide Science 14(2):163-173. [ PubMed : 18085720 ]
  • Ter-Pogossian, M.M, M.E. Phelps, and E.J. Hoffman. 1975. A positron-emission transaxial tomograph for nuclear imaging (PETT) . Radiology 114(1):89-98. [ PubMed : 1208874 ]
  • Torche, A.M., H. Jouan, P. Le Corre, E. Albina, R. Primault, A. Jestin, and R. Le Verge. 2000. Ex vivo and in situ PLGA microspheres uptake by pig ileal Peyer’s patch segment . International Journal of Pharmaceutics 201(1):15-27. [ PubMed : 10867261 ]
  • Toronov, V.A.W., J.H. Choi, M. Wolf, A. Michalos, E. Gratton, and D. Hueber. 2001. Investigation of human brain hemodynamics by simultaneous near-infrared spectroscopy and functional magnetic resonance imaging . Medical Physics 28(4):521-527. [ PubMed : 11339749 ]
  • Tsao, Nina, Puthuparampil P. Kanakamma, Tien-Yau Luh, Chen-Kung Chou, and Huan-Yao Lei. 1999. Inhibition of Escherichia coli -induced meningitis by carboxyfullerene . Antimicrobial Agents and Chemotherapy 43(9):2273-2277. [ PMC free article : PMC89460 ] [ PubMed : 10471578 ]
  • Tsao, Nina, Tien-Yau Luh, Chen-Kung Chou, Jiunn-Jong Wu, Yee-Shin Lin, and Huan-Yao Lei. 2001. Inhibition of group A streptococcus infection by carboxyfullerene . Antimicrobial Agents and Chemotherapy 45(6):1788-1793. [ PMC free article : PMC90546 ] [ PubMed : 11353626 ]
  • Tsiamyrtzis P., J. Dowdall, D. Shastri, I.T. Pavlidis, M.G. Frank, and P. Ekman. 2007. Imaging facial physiology for the detection of deceit . International Journal of Computer Vision 71(2): 197-214.
  • Uvnäs-Moberg, Kerstin. 1998. Oxytocin may mediate the benefits of positive social interaction and emotions . Psychoneuroendocrinology 23(8):819-835. [ PubMed : 9924739 ]
  • Van Der Lubben, I.M., F.A. Konings, G. Borchard, J.C. Verhoef, and H.E. Junginger. 2001. In vivo uptake of chitosan microparticles by murine Peyer’s patches: Visualization studies using confocal laser scanning microscopy and immunohistochemistry . Journal of Drug Targeting 9(1):39-47. [ PubMed : 11378522 ]
  • Varde, Neelesh K., and Daniel W. Pack. 2004. Microspheres for controlled release drug delivery . Expert Opinion on Biological Therapy 4(1):35-51. [ PubMed : 14680467 ]
  • Weissleder, Ralph, Kimberly Kelly, Eric Yi Sun, Timur Shtatland, and Lee Josephson. 2005. Cell-specific targeting of nanoparticles by multivalent attachment of small molecules . Nature Bio technology 23(11):1418-1423. [ PubMed : 16244656 ]
  • White, Barbara Prudhomme, Kathryn A. Becker-Blease, and Kathleen Grace-Bishop. 2006. Stimulant medication use, misuse, and abuse in an undergraduate and graduate student sample . Journal of American College Health 54(5):261-268. [ PubMed : 16539218 ]
  • Wilson, Edward O. 1998. Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge . New York, NY: Knopf Publishing Group.
  • Young, Larry J., Miranda M. Lim, Brenden Gingrich, and Thomas R. Insel. 2001. Cellular mechanisms of social attachment . Hormones and Behavior 40(2):133-138. [ PubMed : 11534973 ]
  • Zak, Paul J., and Ahlam Fakhar. 2006. Neuroactive hormones and interpersonal trust: International evidence . Economics and Human Biology 4(3):412-429. [ PubMed : 16979392 ]
  • Zak, Paul J., Angela A. Stanton, and Sheila Ahmadi. 2007. Oxytocin increases generosity in humans . PLoS ONE 2(11):e1128. [ PMC free article : PMC2040517 ] [ PubMed : 17987115 ]

Unpublished

  • Barrett, James. “Trends and Developments in Neuropsychopharmacology: A 20-year Perspective.” Presentation to the committee on August 15, 2007.
  • Chatterjee, Anjan. “Cosmetic Neurology.” Presentation to the committee on August 15, 2007.
  • Dinges, David. “Optimizing Neurobehavioral Performance though Biology and Technology.” Presentation to the committee on August 15, 2007.
  • Imrey, Peter. “Signal Detection Theory: How Bayes’ Theorem Constrains Accuracy of Inference from Neurophysiologic Monitoring.” Presentation to the committee on October 31, 2007.
  • Kelly, Tom. “Experimental Methods in Clinical Neuropsychopharmacology.” Presentation to the committee on August 15, 2007.
  • Miller, Mike. “Unique Patterns of Individual Brain Activity.” Presentation to the committee on October 31, 2007.
  • Raichle, Marcus. 2007. “Two Views of Brain Function.” Presentation to the committee on October 31, 2007.
  • Simon, Denis. 2007. “Scientific Innovation and Investment in China.” Presentation to the committee on August 15, 2007.
  • Taleb, Nassim Nicholas. 2007. b. Nassim Nicholas Taleb’s home page 2007 [cited December 13, 2007]. Available from http://www ​.fooledbyrandomness.com/ . Last accessed on June 18, 2008.

Individual psychology is also determined by important factors such as fundamental biological drives and programming of behavior, cognition, and affect by all levels of biology, including genes, proteins, receptors, synapses, and nuclei, among others. In addition, endogenous and genetic drivers dominate cognition, affect, and behavioral capability—for example, in human development, sleep and circadian rhythms of cognition and affect, eating, the need for social affiliation and for salt and water, sexual drive, aggression, and nurturing—and dominate human behavior. This section of the report discusses in some detail environmental factors relating to individual psychology, but this is not meant to de-emphasize the importance of biological factors such as the ones just described.

“Symmetric” means “If A then B *and* if B then A.” For example, if there is a symmetric relationship between a lie (A) and a neurophysiological response (B) then every time the lie occurs the specific neurophysiological response occurs *and* every time the neurophysiological response occurs the lie occurs. “Asymmetric” means “If A, then B, but not vice versa.” For example, if a lie is accompanied by a neurophysiological response that does not mean every time the neurophysiological response occurs that a lie has occurred.

In the experimental literature, and especially in the lie detection literature, tacit assumption of 50 percent appears to guide much of the physiological indexing of psychological states. The committee notes this rate may seem otherwise high.

The term “black swan” as defined by Taleb (2007a,b) is an accepted term in the IC for unanticipated consequences. It should be noted that Taleb’s “black swan” is not related to the older term “white crow” that implies sufficiency to disprove a hypothesis.

See http://www ​.sfn.org/index ​.cfm?pagename=annualMeeting ​_statistics&section ​=annualMeeting . Last accessed December 17, 2007.

Under current FDA guidelines, manufacturers are prohibited from marketing approved drugs for off-label use.

For additional information, see http://www ​.cancer.gov . Last accessed on January 24, 2008.

Psivida Ltd. of Australia and SkyePharma of Great Britain are two examples of international interest in this area of research. For additional information, see http://www ​.psivida.com/ . Last accessed on April 10, 2008.

In Cuba, some scientists are reportedly doing very accurate localization using high-density EEG arrays to locate tumors. Personal communication to committee member Scott Bunce from Roy John.

The main injuries during MRI are caused by the magnetic field being attracted by the ferrous (i.e., magnetic) substances within the body. Proper screening of subjects by attending personnel eliminates this risk.

Personal communication between committee chair Christopher Green and Amy Kohl of Wayne State School of Medicine.

SQUID: Superconducting Quantum Interference Device, supercooled electronic component designed to detect extremely small changes in magnetic fields.

Currently, NIRs can localize hemodynamic changes within about 1 cm while the best fMRI scanner can localize changes within a few millimeters.

  • Cite this Page National Research Council (US) Committee on Military and Intelligence Methodology for Emergent Neruophysiological and Cognitive/Neural Research in the Next Two Decades. Emerging Cognitive Neuroscience and Related Technologies. Washington (DC): National Academies Press (US); 2008. 2, Current Cognitive Neuroscience Research and Technology: Selected Areas of Interest.
  • PDF version of this title (4.2M)

In this Page

Related information.

  • PMC PubMed Central citations
  • PubMed Links to PubMed

Recent Activity

  • Current Cognitive Neuroscience Research and Technology: Selected Areas of Intere... Current Cognitive Neuroscience Research and Technology: Selected Areas of Interest - Emerging Cognitive Neuroscience and Related Technologies

Your browsing activity is empty.

Activity recording is turned off.

Turn recording back on

Connect with NLM

National Library of Medicine 8600 Rockville Pike Bethesda, MD 20894

Web Policies FOIA HHS Vulnerability Disclosure

Help Accessibility Careers

statistics

Department of Psychology

Dietrich college of humanities and social sciences.

100 Years CMU Psychology

Cognitive Science

Our graduate program focuses on training students to become independent scientists in cognitive psychology..

Faculty train researchers to develop a deep understanding of representations and processes involved in perceiving, thinking, and acting at functional, computational, and neural levels.

Related Links

  • Program Requirements
  • Cognitive Science Research

These faculty routinely train Cognitive Science students:

John Anderson headshot

John Anderson

Richard King Mellon University Professor of Psychology and Computer Science

We study learning and performance in complex tasks like mathematical problem solving and video games.  To test our understanding we build computational models that can perform these tasks in the ACT-R cognitive architecture.

Marlene Behrmann headshot

Marlene Behrmann

Thomas S. Baker University Professor of Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience

Behrmann's research program is on understanding the psychological and neural bases of visual cognition, with particular emphasis on the recognition of faces, words and common objects. The research involves a multimodal approach using psychophysics, functional MRI and EEG and different populations including normal and brain-damaged adults and children.

Jessica Cantlon headshot

Jessica Cantlon

Ronald J. and Mary Ann Zdrojkowski Associate Professor of Developmental Neuroscience/Psychology

We study the developmental, evolutionary, and cultural origins of logic and mathematics using fMRI and behavioral methods with children and adults, comparative studies with non-human primates, and cross-cultural research in the Amazon. Our studies test the kinds of computations that are shared among primates, their developmental trajectory, and what makes the human brain unique.

Gretchen Chapman headshot

Gretchen Chapman

Professor of Social and Decision Sciences and Professor of Psychology (Courtesy)

Dr. Chapman’s research combines the fields of judgment and decision making with health psychology to investigate methods to facilitate healthy and prosocial behavior such as vaccination and blood donation.

Anna Fisher headshot

Anna Fisher

Associate Professor of Psychology

I study development of attention regulation, role of attention in learning in formal and informal settings, and development of higher-order cognition including semantic memory and reasoning. On-going projects in the lab examine how children sustain attention to dynamic events, the role children’s developing attention regulation plays during reading, and learning-driven changes in semantic memory in ecologically-valid settings.

Laurie Heller headshot

Laurie Heller

Professor of Psychology (Teaching)

My research examines the human ability to use sound to understand what events are happening in the environment. My perceptual experiments address whether there are acoustic cues that reveal attributes of sound events, and how our knowledge of these cue-attribute relationships influences our discrimination of sounds, labeling of sounds, and multimodal perception.

Roberta Klatzky headshot

Roberta Klatzky

Charles J. Queenan, Jr. University Professor of Psychology and Human-Computer Interaction

I study how humans form perceptual representations in multiple sensory modalities and use them to guide action. My work has a strong connection to application, such as aiding blind people in navigation or augmenting surgery with touch and visual displays.

Ken Koedinger headshot

Ken Koedinger

Hillman Professor of Computer Science and Professor of Human-Computer Interaction and Psychology

I study the great mystery of how complex human learning works, empirically using fine-grained longitudinal data from math, science, and language learning, experimentally through studies to identify causal relationships that lead to improved learning, and theoretically using machine learning to model how people learn to behave, reason, and problem solve.

Marsha Lovett headshot

Marsha Lovett

Associate Vice Provost for Educational Innovation and Learning Analytics, Director of Eberly Center for Teaching Excellence & Educational Innovation, Co-coordinator of The Simon Initiative and Professor of Psychology (Teaching)

I study how learning works (mostly in college-level courses) and then explore ways to improve it. I work with learning data from online technologies and classroom-based sensors and have developed several educational technologies to support this work.

Brian MacWhinney headshot

Brian MacWhinney

Teresa Heinz Professor of Cognitive Psychology

I have created the crosslinguistic multimodal TalkBank database system for the study of spoken language interactions in children, adults, bilinguals, language learners, and a variety of language disorders. My Competition Model for first and second language learning relies on concepts and data from usage-based linguistics, embodied cognition, and neuroemergentism .

Brad Mahon headshot

I work on the organization of object concepts in the brain, and how the brain recovers from injury.

Daniel Oppenheimer headshot

Daniel Oppenheimer

I study how do people determine what information to use when making decisions, what is worth learning, how to search for the information they need, and what people do when different pieces of information conflict and suggest different conclusions.  To answer these questions, I integrate theory from metacognition, causal reasoning, and memory.

David Plaut headshot

David Plaut

Professor of Psychology and the Neuroscience Institute

My research involves using computational (neural-network) modeling, complemented by behavioral, neuropsychological, and neuroimaging studies, to investigate the neural basis of cognitive processing in the domains of high-level vision, reading and language, and semantics, over the course of development, in skilled adults, and in patients with brain damage.

Lynne Reder headshot

Lynne Reder

Professor of Psychology

I strive to better understand human memory by combining behavioral, neuroimaging, psychopharmacology and computational modeling methods to help constrain my theorizing. Currently, I am exploring how experience with stimuli affects working memory consumption and its implications for learning and partial matching.

Michael J. Tarr headshot

Michael J. Tarr

Kavčić-Moura Professor of Cognitive and Brain Science, Head of the Department of Psychology, Professor of Psychology and the Neuroscience Institute and Professor of Machine Learning (Courtesy)

Using tools drawn from machine learning, computer vision, computer graphics, cognitive science and neuroimaging we are exploring how high-level structures within biological vision systems arise given minimal conditions, as well as how such models can help us to better understand the processes that underlie vision.

Erik Thiessen headshot

Erik Thiessen

Director of Undergraduate Studies and Associate Professor of Psychology

I study learning, with a particular focus on language learning and how infants and adults take advantage of probabilistic information in the input to discover phonological, lexical, and syntactic patterns.  I am interested in both the effects of learning - such as how adults process input differently from infants - and also the mechanisms underlying learning, and have ongoing projects exploring both of these questions.

Timothy Verstynen headshot

Timothy Verstynen

Associate Professor of Psychology and the Neuroscience Institute

My lab studies how the architecture of cortical and subcortical circuits gives rise to the algorithms of decision-making and learning. We apply this understanding in the context of health neuroscience and artificial intelligence.

  • Community Standards
  • Faculty Resources

Cognitive Psychology

The faculty in cognitive psychology have research programs focused on human memory, language processing, categorization, reasoning, computational modeling, and cognitive aging. The cognitive psychology program emphasizes acquiring exceptionally strong research skills in a domain chosen by the student, working closely with individual faculty members, and using multiple dependent measures to study a given area. Faculty members and students in the cognitive psychology program have research interests and collaborations that overlap with those in every other program area in the Department. In addition to strong intersections with the  Attention and Perception  and  Cognitive Neuroscience  programs, there are also collaborations with social, developmental, and quantitative psychologists.

Cognitive psychology faculty offer a large array of graduate courses that cover the field of cognitive psychology in an exceptionally comprehensive manner. Students develop a specialized course of study designed to provide expertise in their particular areas of interest, but also acquire breadth in the field through coursework outside the program area as well as through interdisciplinary collaborations. All students complete a first-year project that is presented to the faculty at a weekly research seminar attended by all students and faculty in the program. By the end of the third year, students will have completed the Comprehensive Examination as well as the majority of their coursework. The material to be covered on the Comprehensive Examination is jointly decided by the student and his or her faculty advisor. The remaining years in the program are devoted to completing a dissertation, publishing data collected earlier, as well as developing depth and breadth in preparation for a career in academia, industry, or government service, with the majority of students entering academia.

Facilities and Resources

The facilities associated with cognitive psychology are superlative. Every faculty member has substantial laboratory space dedicated to his or her research program in either the Psychology Department or Beckman Institute. All students have access to a vast array of state-of-the-art technologies and methodologies as well as a high-end computing environment. Tools available to study cognition, in addition to traditional behavioral methods, include a research-dedicated head-only 3 Tesla scanner for functional neuroimaging, as well as optical imaging, EEG/ERP, and eye-tracking. Students have access to many different subject populations, including older adults, children, and clinical populations. The technical resources available to students in cognitive psychology are among the most comprehensive and best in the world for the study of human cognition.

Affiliated Departments, Programs, and Institutes

Most faculty members in cognitive psychology have appointments in the  Beckman Institute  for Advanced Science and Technology. In addition, faculty members collaborate with faculty members in other Departments at UIUC, including Linguistics, Engineering, Educational Psychology, Kinesiology, the Genomics Institute.

Faculty working in Cognitive Psychology

Profile picture for Aaron S Benjamin

Related News

Photograph of Catharine Fairbairn (left) and Kara Federmeier (right)

Related Content

  • Summer II 2024 Research Assistants for Speech Production Project We are seeking responsible, highly motivated students with an interest in language and learning to assist with the research project in the Language Acquisition Lab that’s currently ongoing for Summer...
  • 23-24 Brain and Cognitive Development Lab Research Assistants (2) The BCDLab is looking for 1-2 more undergraduate research assistants for the upcoming school year.  We study cognition and development using optical (fNIRS) and electrophysiological (ERP/EEG) brain...
  • 23-24 Paid Research Coordinator Position-Brain and Cognitive Dev. Lab   The Brain and Cognitive Development Lab at the University of Illinois Urbana-Champaign is seeking a qualified individual to work as a research coordinator.  The lab uses behavioral,...

Eye tracker and text displayed in three different parts of the visual field

  • Spring-Summer 2023 Research Assistant - Image Semantics project, Learning and Language Lab In the Image Semantics project within the Learning and Language Lab, we are analyzing a video dataset gathered with headcams worn by infants and children, capturing their first person perspectives. ...
  • Spring 2023 Research Assistant - Daylong Language Environment project, Learning and Language Lab In the Daylong Language Environment project we transcribe, code, and analyze audio data that captures the everyday language environments of infants and children through recorders they wore at home. ...
  • Fall 2022 Research Assistant - Picture Book Reading project, Learning and Language Lab We are analyzing audio recordings made by parents who read picture books to their children at home. We transcribe the recordings to understand the language children experience in their home...
  • Fall 2022 Research Assistant - Image Semantics project, Learning and Language Lab In the Image Semantics project we are analyzing a cool video dataset gathered with headcams worn by infants and children, capturing their first person perspectives.  We use these videos to get...
  • Fall 2020 Research Assistant - SCOPE Neuroscience Lab The Social, Cognitive, Personality, and Emotion Neuroscience Lab is seeking to recruit undergraduate students for PSYC 290. You can learn more about our research by visiting...
  • Fall 2022 Research Assistant - Daylong Language Environment project, Learning and Language Lab In the Daylong Language Environment project we transcribe, code, and analyze audio data that captures the everyday language environments of infants and children through recorders they wore at home. ...
  • Spring 2022 Research Assistant for the "Learning language from the environment" project in Learning and Language Lab We are analyzing audio recordings made by parents who read picture books to their children at home. We transcribe the recordings to understand the language children experience in their home...
  • Spring 2022 Research Assistant for the Learning and Language Lab Studying Toddlers Everyday Experiences We are analyzing a cool dataset of videos gathered with headcams worn by infants and children, capturing their first person perspectives of their early experiences.  We code these videos to get...

Image on the left shows 3 brightly colored clip-art pictures on a white background; image on the right shows a cute preschool-aged girl with curly hair.

  • Spring 2021 Research Assistant for a Sentence Processing Project We are looking for one or two research assistants to assist with projects investigating sentence processing and sentence production. This position will be entirely online. Research assistants will...
  • Cognitive Psychology Concentration
  • Research News
  • Attention & Perception
  • Behavioral Neuroscience
  • Clinical-Community
  • Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Developmental
  • Industrial-Organizational
  • Quantitative
  • Social-Personality
  • Research Labs
  • Undergraduate Research Opportunities
  • SONA Studies and Paid Experiments
  • Illinois Experts Research Portal

Encyclopedia Britannica

  • Games & Quizzes
  • History & Society
  • Science & Tech
  • Biographies
  • Animals & Nature
  • Geography & Travel
  • Arts & Culture
  • On This Day
  • One Good Fact
  • New Articles
  • Lifestyles & Social Issues
  • Philosophy & Religion
  • Politics, Law & Government
  • World History
  • Health & Medicine
  • Browse Biographies
  • Birds, Reptiles & Other Vertebrates
  • Bugs, Mollusks & Other Invertebrates
  • Environment
  • Fossils & Geologic Time
  • Entertainment & Pop Culture
  • Sports & Recreation
  • Visual Arts
  • Demystified
  • Image Galleries
  • Infographics
  • Top Questions
  • Britannica Kids
  • Saving Earth
  • Space Next 50
  • Student Center
  • When did science begin?
  • Where was science invented?

Blackboard inscribed with scientific formulas and calculations in physics and mathematics

cognitive psychology

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

  • CORE - Integrating Cognitive Ethology with Cognitive Psychology
  • The U4 Anti-Corruption Resource Centre - Cognitive psychology concepts for understanding corrupt behaviour
  • Verywell Mind - Cognitive Psychology
  • Pressbooks - Cognition
  • Social Science LibreTexts - History of Cognitive Psychology
  • National Center for Biotechnology Information - PubMed Central - How cognitive psychology changed the face of medical education research
  • Simply Psychology - Cognitive Approach in Psychology
  • WebMD - What is Cognitive Psychology?

cognitive psychology , Branch of psychology devoted to the study of human cognition , particularly as it affects learning and behaviour. The field grew out of advances in Gestalt , developmental , and comparative psychology and in computer science , particularly information-processing research. Cognitive psychology shares many research interests with cognitive science , and some experts classify it as a branch of the latter. Contemporary cognitive theory has followed one of two broad approaches: the developmental approach, derived from the work of Jean Piaget and concerned with “representational thought” and the construction of mental models (“schemas”) of the world, and the information-processing approach, which views the human mind as analogous to a sophisticated computer system .

What Is the Cognitive Psychology Approach? 12 Key Theories

Cognitive Psychology

Maintaining focus on the oncoming traffic is paramount, yet I am barely aware of the seagulls flying overhead.

These noisy birds only receive attention when I am safely walking up the other side of the road, their cries reminding me of childhood seaside vacations.

Cognitive psychology focuses on the internal mental processes needed to make sense of the environment and decide on the next appropriate action (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

This article explores the cognitive psychology approach, its origins, and several theories and models involved in cognition.

Before you continue, we thought you might like to download our three Positive Psychology Exercises for free . These science-based exercises explore fundamental aspects of positive psychology, including strengths, values, and self-compassion, and will give you the tools to enhance the wellbeing of your clients, students, or employees.

This Article Contains:

What is the cognitive psychology approach, a brief history of cognitive psychology, cognitive psychology vs behaviorism, 12 key theories, concepts, and models, fascinating research experiments, a look at positive cognitive psychology, interesting resources from positivepsychology.com, a take-home message.

The upsurge of research into the mysteries of the human brain and mind has been considerable in recent decades, with recognition of the importance of cognitive process in clinical psychology and social psychology  (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

As a result, cognitive psychology has profoundly affected the field of psychology and our understanding of what it is to be human.

Perhaps more surprisingly, it has had such an effect without clear boundaries, an integrated set of assumptions and concepts, or a recognizable spokesperson (Gross, 2020).

So, what exactly is the cognitive psychology approach?

Cognitive psychology attempts to understand human cognition by focusing on what appear to be cognitive tasks that require little effort (Goldstein, 2011).

Let’s return to our example of walking down the road. Imagine now that we are also taking a call. We’re now combining several concurrent cognitive tasks:

  • Perceiving the environment Distinguishing cars from traffic signals and discerning their direction and speed on the road as well as the people ahead standing, talking, and blocking the sidewalk.
  • Paying attention Attending to what our partner is asking us on the phone, above the traffic noise.
  • Visualizing Forming a mental image of items in the house, responding to the question, “Where did you leave your car keys?”
  • Comprehending and producing language Understanding the real question (“I need to take the car. Where are your keys?”) from what is said and formulating a suitable reply.
  • Problem-solving Working out how to get to the next appointment without the car.
  • Decision-making Concluding that the timing of one meeting will not work and choosing to push it to another day.

While cognitive psychologists initially focused firmly on an analogy comparing the mind to a computer, their understanding has moved on.

There are currently four approaches, often overlapping and frequently combined, that science uses to understand human cognition (Eysenck & Keane, 2015):

  • Cognitive psychology The attempt to “understand human cognition by using behavioral evidence” (Eysenck & Keane, 2015, p. 2).
  • Cognitive neuropsychology Understanding ‘normal’ cognition through the study of patients living with a brain injury.
  • Cognitive neuroscience Combining evidence from the brain with behavior to form a more complete picture of cognition.
  • Computational cognitive science Using computational models to understand and test our understanding of human cognition.

Cognitive psychology plays a massive and essential role in understanding human cognition and is stronger because of its close relationships and interdependencies with other academic disciplines (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

History of Cognitive Psychology

In 1868, a Dutch physiologist, Franciscus Donders, began to measure reaction time – something we would now see as an experiment in cognitive psychology (Goldstein, 2011).

Donders recognized that mental responses could not be measured directly but could be inferred from behavior. Not long after, Hermann Ebbinghaus began examining the nature and inner workings of human memory using nonsense syllables (Goldstein, 2011).

By the late 1800s, Wilhelm Wundt had set up the first laboratory dedicated to studying the mind scientifically. His approach became known as structuralism . His bold aim was to build a periodic table of the mind , containing all the sensations involved in creating any experience (Goldstein, 2011).

However, the use of analytical introspection to uncover hidden mental processes was gradually dropped when John Watson proposed a new psychological approach that became known as behaviorism (Goldstein, 2011).

Watson rejected the introspective approach and instead focused on observable behavior. His idea of classical conditioning – the connection of a new stimulus with a previously neutral one – was later surpassed by B. F. Skinner’s idea of operant conditioning , which focused on positive reinforcement (Goldstein, 2011).

Both theories sought to understand the relationship between stimulus and response rather than the mind’s inner workings (Goldstein, 2011).

Prompted by a scathing attack by linguist and cognitive scientist Noam Chomsky, by the 1950s behaviorism as the dominant psychological discipline was in decline. The introduction of the digital computer led to the information-processing approach , inspiring psychologists to think of the mind in terms of a sequence of processing stages (Goldstein, 2011).

3 positive psychology exercises

Download 3 Free Positive Psychology Exercises (PDF)

Enhance wellbeing with these free, science-based exercises that draw on the latest insights from positive psychology.

Download 3 Free Positive Psychology Tools Pack (PDF)

By filling out your name and email address below.

Moore (1996) recognized the tensions of the paradigm shift from behaviorism to cognitive psychology.

While research into cognitive psychology, cognitive neuropsychology, cognitive neuroscience , and computational cognitive science is now widely accepted as the driving force behind understanding mental processes (such as memory, perception, problem-solving, and attention), this was not always the case (Gross, 2020).

Moore (1996) highlighted the relationship between behaviorism and the relatively new field of cognitive psychology, and the sometimes mistaken assumptions regarding the nature of the former approach:

  • Behaviorism is typically only associated with studying publicly observable behavior. Unlike behaviorism, cognitive psychology is viewed as free of the restrictions of logical positivism, which rely on verification through observation.

Since then, modern cognitive psychology has incorporated findings from many other disciplines, including evolutionary psychology , computer science, artificial intelligence , and neuroscience (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

  • Unlike behaviorism, cognitive psychology is theoretical and explanatory. Behaviorism is often considered merely descriptive, while cognitive psychology is seen as being able to explain what is behind behavior.

Particular ongoing advances in cognitive psychology include perception, language comprehension and production, and problem-solving (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

  • Behaviorism cannot incorporate theoretical terms. While challenged by some behaviorists at the time, it was argued that behaviorism could not incorporate theoretical terms unless related to directly observable behavior.

At the time, cognitive psychologists also argued that it was wrong of behaviorists to interpret mental states in terms of brain states.

Neuroscience advances, such as new imaging techniques like functional MRI, continue to offer fresh insights into the relationship between the brain and mental states (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Clearly, the relationship between behaviorism and the developing field of cognitive psychology has been complex. However, cognitive psychology has grown into a school of thought that has led to significant advances in understanding cognition, especially when teamed up with other developments in computing and neuroscience.

This may not have been possible without the shift in the dominant schools of thought in psychology (Gross, 2020; Goldstein, 2011; Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Cognitive Psychology Theories

And while it is beyond the scope of this article to cover the full breadth or depth of the areas of research, we list several of the most important and fascinating specialties and theories below.

It is hardly possible to imagine a world in which attention doesn’t play an essential role in how we interact with the environment, and yet, we rarely give it a thought.

According to cognitive psychology, attention is most active when driven by an individual’s expectations or goals, known as top-down processing . On the other hand, it is more passive when controlled by external stimuli, such as a loud noise, referred to as bottom-up processing (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

A further distinction exists between focused attention (selective) and divided attention . Research into the former explores how we are able to focus on one item (noise, image, etc.) when there are several. In contrast, the latter looks at how we can maintain attention on two or more stimuli simultaneously.

Donald Broadbent proposed the bottleneck model to explain how we can attend to just one message when several are presented, for example, in dichotic listening experiments, where different auditory stimuli are presented to each ear. Broadbent’s model suggests multiple processing stages, each one progressively restricting the information flow (Goldstein, 2011).

As with all other areas of cognition, perception is far more complicated than we might first imagine. Take, for example, vision. While a great deal of research has “involved presenting a visual stimulus and assessing aspects of its processing,” there is also the time aspect to consider (Eysenck & Keane, 2015, p. 121).

We need to not only perceive objects, but also make sense of their movement and detect changes in the visual environment over time (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Research suggests perception, like attention, combines bottom-up and top-down processing. Bottom-up processing involves neurons that fire in response to specific elements of an image – perhaps aspects of a face, nose, eyebrows, jawline, etc. Top-down processing considers how the knowledge someone brings with them affects their perception.

Bottom-down processing helps explain why two people, presented with the same stimuli, experience different perceptions as a result of their expectations and prior knowledge (Goldstein, 2011).

Combining bottom-up and top-down processing also enables the individual to make sense of both static and moving images when limited information is available; we can track a person walking through a crowd or a plane disappearing in and out of clouds (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

The mirror neuron system is incredibly fascinating and is proving valuable in our attempts to understand biological motion. Observing actions activates similar areas of the brain as performing them. The model appears to explain how we can imitate the actions of another person – crucial to learning (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Language comprehension

Whether written or spoken, understanding language involves a high degree of multi-level processing (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Comprehension begins with an initial analysis of sentence structure (larger language units require additional processing). Beyond processing syntax (the rules for building and analyzing sentences), analysis of sentence meaning ( semantics ) is necessary to understand if the interpretation should be literal or involve irony, metaphor, or sarcasm (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Pragmatics examines intended meaning. For example, shouting, “That’s the doorbell!” is not likely to be a simple observation, but rather a request to answer the door (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Several models have been proposed to understand the analysis and comprehension of sentences, known as parsing , including (Eysenck & Keane, 2015):

  • Garden-path model This model attempts to explain why some sentences are ambiguous (such as, “The horse raced past the barn fell.”). It suggests they are challenging to comprehend because the analysis is performed on each individual unit of the sentence with little feedback, and correction is inhibited.
  • Constraint-based model The interpretations of a sentence may be limited by several constraints, including syntactic, semantic, and general world knowledge.
  • Unrestricted race model This model combines the garden-path and constraint-based model, and suggests all sources of information inform syntactic structure. One such interpretation is selected until it is discarded, with good reason, for another.
  • Good-enough representation This model proposes that parsing provides a ‘good-enough’ interpretation rather than something detailed, accurate, and complete.

The research and theories above hint at the vast complexity of human cognition and explain why so many models and concepts attempt to answer what happens when it works and, equally important, when it doesn’t.

A level of psychology: the cognitive approach – Atomi

There are many research experiments in cognitive psychology that highlight the successes and failings of human cognition. Each of the following three offers insight into the mental processes behind our thinking and behavior.

Cocktail party phenomenon

Selective attention – or in this case, selective listening – is often exemplified by what has become known as the cocktail party phenomenon  (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Even in a busy room and possibly mid-conversation, we can often hear if someone else mentions our name. It seems we can filter out surrounding noise by combining bottom-up and top-down processing to create a “winner takes it all” situation where the processing of one high-value auditory input suppresses the brain activity of all others (Goldstein, 2011).

While people may believe that the speed of hand movement allows magicians to trick us, research suggests the main factor is misdirection (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

A 2010 study of a trick involving the disappearance of a lighter identified that when the lighter was dropped (to hide it from a later hand-opening finale), it was masked by directing attention from the fixation point – known as covert attention – with surprising effectiveness.

However, subjects were able to identify the drop when their attention was directed to the fixation point – known as overt attention (Kuhn & Findlay, 2010).

In a thought-provoking study exploring freewill, participants were asked to consciously decide whether to move their finger left or right while a functional MRI scanner monitored their prefrontal cortex and parietal cortex (Soon, Brass, Heinze, & Haynes, 2008).

Brain activity predicted the direction of movement a full seven seconds before they consciously became aware of their decision. While follow-up research has challenged some of the findings, it appears that brain activity may come before conscious thinking (Eysenck & Keane, 2015).

Positive Cognitive Psychology

Associations have been found between positive emotions, creative thinking, and overall wellbeing, suggesting environmental changes that may benefit staff productivity and innovation in the workplace (Yuan, 2015).

Factors explored include creating climates geared toward creativity, boosting challenge, trust, freedom, risk taking, low conflict, and even the beneficial effects of humor.

Undoubtedly, further innovation will be seen from marrying the two powerful and compelling new fields of positive psychology and cognitive psychology.

research areas in cognitive psychology

World’s Largest Positive Psychology Resource

The Positive Psychology Toolkit© is a groundbreaking practitioner resource containing over 500 science-based exercises , activities, interventions, questionnaires, and assessments created by experts using the latest positive psychology research.

Updated monthly. 100% Science-based.

“The best positive psychology resource out there!” — Emiliya Zhivotovskaya , Flourishing Center CEO

We have many tools, worksheets, and exercises to explore and improve attention, problem-solving, and the ability to regulate emotions.

Why not download our free emotional intelligence pack and try out the powerful tools contained within?

  • Building Emotional Awareness In this exercise, we foster emotional intelligence by mindfully attending to existing emotional states.
  • Identifying False Beliefs About Emotions Our beliefs often operate outside of conscious awareness. This exercise addresses clients’ basic and often unconscious assumptions about their emotions.

Other free resources include:

  • Skills for Regulating Emotion We can learn to manage our emotions by focusing on more positive experiences than negative ones.
  • Emotional Repetition and Attention Remodeling Identifying phrases used to describe ourselves can help desensitize negative feelings .

More extensive versions of the following tools are available with a subscription to the Positive Psychology Toolkit© , but here is a brief overview:

  • Creating Savoring Rituals It’s possible to increase positive emotions by sharpening our sensory perceptions via savoring.

Learning to focus can help.

Step one – Identify everyday activities that bring you pleasure. Step two – Focus on experiencing pleasure as it happens when doing these activities.

At the end of the week, take some time to record your reflections on creating savoring rituals.

  • Extracting Strengths From Problems Surprisingly, using our strengths too much can harm our problem-solving ability.

In this exercise, we examine an existing issue in a client’s life:

Step one – Describe a current problem. Step two – Identify the problematic context or life domain. Step three – Identify the problematic behavior in yourself. Step four – Recognize your underlying strength. Step five – Identify what you can do to remedy the problem.

If you’re looking for more science-based ways to help others enhance their wellbeing, check out this signature collection of 17 validated positive psychology tools for practitioners. Use them to help others flourish and thrive.

Cognitive psychology is crucial in our search for understanding how we interact with and make sense of a constantly changing and potentially harmful environment.

Not only that, it offers insight into what happens when things go wrong and the likely impact on our wellbeing and ability to cope with life events.

Cognitive psychology’s strength is its willingness to embrace research findings from many other disciplines, combining them with existing psychological theory to create new models of cognition.

The tasks we appear to carry out unconsciously are a great deal more complex than they might first appear. Perception, attention, problem-solving, language comprehension and production, and decision-making often happen without intentional thought and yet have enormous consequences on our lives.

Use this article as a starting point to explore the many and diverse aspects of cognitive psychology. Consider their relationships with associated research fields and reflect on the importance of understanding cognition in helping clients overcome complex events or circumstances.

We hope you enjoyed reading this article. Don’t forget to download our three Positive Psychology Exercises for free .

  • Eysenck, M. W., & Keane, M. T. (2015). Cognitive psychology: A student’s handbook . Psychology Press.
  • Goldstein, E. B. (2011). Cognitive psychology . Wadsworth, Cengage Learning.
  • Gross, R. D. (2020). Psychology: The science of mind and behaviour . Hodder and Stoughton.
  • Kuhn, G., & Findlay, J. M. (2010). Misdirection, attention and awareness: Inattentional blindness reveals temporal relationship between eye movements and visual awareness. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology , 63 (1), 136–146.
  • Moore, J. (1996). On the relation between behaviorism and cognitive psychology. Journal of Mind and Behavior , 17 (4), 345–367
  • Soon, C. S., Brass, M., Heinze, H., & Haynes, J. (2008). Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain. Nature Neuroscience , 11 (5), 543–545.
  • Yuan, L. (2015). The happier one is, the more creative one becomes: An investigation on inspirational positive emotions from both subjective well-being and satisfaction at work. Psychology , 6 , 201–209.

' src=

Share this article:

Article feedback

What our readers think.

Janice L. Jamrosz

As a widowed Mother and Grandmother, whom was recently told by an adult child that maybe I should have “cognitive” testing done, I found this article to be very informative and refreshing. Having the ability to read and and learn about cognitive psychology is interesting as their are so many ways our brains are affected from the time we are born until the time we reach each and every stage in life. I have spent time with my grandchildren who are from age 19 months, through 15 years old , and spend time with children who are 35, 34, and 32, and my parents who are 88 and 84. I appreciate your article and your time in writing it. Sincerely,

Niranjan Dev Makker

Cognitive Psychology creates & build human capacity to push physical and mental limits. My concept of cognition in human behavior was judged by the most time I met my lawyer or the doctor. Most of the time while listening a pause, oh I see and it is perpetual transition to see. Cognition emergence is very vital support as we see & perceive. My practices in engineering solution are base on my cognitive sensibilities.You article provokes the same perceptions. Thank you

Let us know your thoughts Cancel reply

Your email address will not be published.

Save my name, email, and website in this browser for the next time I comment.

Related articles

Hierarchy of needs

Hierarchy of Needs: A 2024 Take on Maslow’s Findings

One of the most influential theories in human psychology that addresses our quest for wellbeing is Abraham Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs. While Maslow’s theory of [...]

Emotional Development

Emotional Development in Childhood: 3 Theories Explained

We have all witnessed a sweet smile from a baby. That cute little gummy grin that makes us smile in return. Are babies born with [...]

Classical Conditioning Phobias

Using Classical Conditioning for Treating Phobias & Disorders

Does the name Pavlov ring a bell? Classical conditioning, a psychological phenomenon first discovered by Ivan Pavlov in the late 19th century, has proven to [...]

Read other articles by their category

  • Body & Brain (50)
  • Coaching & Application (58)
  • Compassion (25)
  • Counseling (51)
  • Emotional Intelligence (23)
  • Gratitude (18)
  • Grief & Bereavement (21)
  • Happiness & SWB (40)
  • Meaning & Values (26)
  • Meditation (20)
  • Mindfulness (44)
  • Motivation & Goals (45)
  • Optimism & Mindset (34)
  • Positive CBT (30)
  • Positive Communication (22)
  • Positive Education (47)
  • Positive Emotions (32)
  • Positive Leadership (19)
  • Positive Parenting (16)
  • Positive Psychology (34)
  • Positive Workplace (37)
  • Productivity (18)
  • Relationships (44)
  • Resilience & Coping (39)
  • Self Awareness (21)
  • Self Esteem (38)
  • Strengths & Virtues (32)
  • Stress & Burnout Prevention (34)
  • Theory & Books (46)
  • Therapy Exercises (37)
  • Types of Therapy (64)

3 Positive Psychology Tools (PDF)

research areas in cognitive psychology

Areas of Research

graphic icons representing areas of neuroscience research

The MIT Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences has an ambitious mission: to understand how the mechanisms of the brain give rise to the mind. To advance this vision, we bring together researchers, students, and faculty who study brain science at all levels.

Our researchers often cross the boundaries of established fields, or invent new disciplines entirely. Conceptually, however, we think of our research in four broad categories:

research areas in cognitive psychology

BCS faculty who are conducting research in this area can be found here.

Research in cellular and molecular neuroscience strives to understand the brain at its most fundamental level by studying the mechanisms that control construction and maintenance of cellular and molecular circuits.

Work in this area creates a window into how neurons are born and migrate, and how they form synaptic connections. Understanding how synapses function and undergo plasticity also allows insights into the molecular underpinnings of memory formation in the brain. Studying the ways that neurons operate will move us closer to understanding how the brain develops and responds to outside stimuli. The interplay of the complex molecular machinery of the neuronal membrane with the dynamics of electrical potentials is critical to understanding the synaptic contacts where neurons communicate with each other. This leads to important questions at the systems level. The plasticity of these contacts, expressed by neuronal axons, allows robust behavioral modification to changing environmental stimuli and internal representations. 

Disruptions of the molecular machines that underlie neuronal development and function are also at the heart of most neurological and psychiatric diseases. This provides strong motivation to define how these molecular and cellular pathways allow neurons to connect and communicate, and how they go awry in brain diseases.

Cellular and molecular neuroscience is a deep mystery, but it brings exciting and critical bridges to other facets of brain and cognitive science. Researchers at BCS are using the latest tools and technologies to unlock critical applications of molecular science, including the prospects of future genetic intervention that might one day lead to cures for brain diseases. 

Our focus in these important areas will help bring about new treatments for both neurodevelopment diseases like autism, as well as late-onset neurodegenerative diseases like Alzheimer’s. These studies also promise new insights into how other brain-related disorders associated with aging alter the functional interplay of neuronal function and connectivity.

research areas in cognitive psychology

In systems neuroscience, researchers use animal models to emulate core cognitive processes. This allows for more detailed study of algorithms and neural circuits that produce the representations of the mind. Scientists examine how patterns of neuronal connections (circuits) give rise to patterns of neuronal activity, and how those patterns of neural activity give rise to overt behavioral and different internal neural states.

Systems neuroscience studies the processes that occur within our central nervous system. Animal models allow much more precise study and intervention in the neural circuits that underlie higher cognitive function. Although these models do not capture the full mental abilities of humans, they are selected such that they likely share evolutionarily conserved neuronal processing mechanisms that will generalize to human brain function.  

This research is important to all aspects of our work. It provides detailed data that is used to build computational models of cognitive processes. It also allows us to test hypotheses about brain function by precisely intervening in the system in ways that are not possible in humans, such as neural or genetic manipulations. 

These experiments are critical to building our understanding, as captured by computational models. They are also central to our exploration of possible ways to repair or augment broken neural circuits in diseased or disrupted states. 

Because systems neuroscientists seek to understand the basis for cognitive, motivational, sensory, and motor processes, their work overlaps with that of our other research disciplines.  These connections are critical in uncovering answers to basic questions about how we move, learn and feel. 

research areas in cognitive psychology

Cognitive science is the scientific study of the human mind. It is a highly interdisciplinary field, combining ideas and methods from psychology, computer science, linguistics, philosophy, and neuroscience. The broad goal of cognitive science is to characterize the nature of human knowledge – its forms and content – and how that knowledge is used, processed, and acquired. 

Active areas of cognitive research in the Department include language, memory, visual perception and cognition, thinking and reasoning, social cognition, decision making, and cognitive development. 

The study of cognitive science within BCS illustrates the department’s philosophy that understanding the mind and understanding the brain are ultimately inseparable, even with the gaps that currently exist between the core questions of human cognition and the questions that can be productively addressed in molecular, cellular or systems neuroscience. To bridge these gaps, several cognitive labs maintain a primary or secondary focus on cognitive neuroscience research. There are many opportunities for interaction and collaboration between cognitive and neuroscience labs across BCS and its related centers.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Computational neuroscience uses the tools of mathematics and computers to develop theoretical models that test and expand our understanding of the workings of brain and behavioral processes. Unlike the related field of artificial intelligence, computation seeks not just to create intelligence out of machines, but to illuminate the processes that underlie sensation and perception, control of action, learning and memory, language, and other cognitive processes. 

These theoretical studies offer the prospect of connecting diverse research constructs and paradigms, and of providing a new understanding of the algorithms that drive our “mental machinery.” 

BCS scientists are focused on three key areas of computation:

  • The study of the data representations and algorithms that autonomous systems might build to perform tasks that are important for human survival (closely related to artificial intelligence).
  • The implementation and testing of circuits that are constrained by neuronal data but aim to accomplish the tasks above.  
  • The development of analysis and statistical tools for analyzing and visualizing neuroscience data. 

Understanding something as complex as the human mind requires computational models that accurately translate the system’s internal workings. Models help us build formal bridges between any two levels of analysis. For example: from gene expression programs to regulation of neuronal connections (synapses), or from neuronal circuit connections to patterns of neuronal activity. Other examples include from patterns of neuronal activity to behavioral report and mental states, and last, from mental states to cognitive function. 

As we work to build a complete picture of the neural mechanisms of the mind, it is necessary for us to link models of all levels. Models allow us to make predictions about behavior, to emulate key aspects of neural computations in other devices (brain inspired computing), and to consider the best ways to repair or augment key functions.  

Site Logo

Psychology Research Areas

The UC Davis Department of Psychology contains five major "areas": Developmental, Perception-Cognition, Biological Psychology, Quantitative and Social-Personality. Boundaries between the areas are fluid, and students are encouraged to take seminars in all five.

  • Developmental Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology includes imaging the developing brain, children's psychological understanding or theory of mind (e.g., knowledge about people's thoughts, beliefs, desires, and emotions), children’s language development, and children's social development Read More
  • Social and Personality Psychology
  • Social and Personality Psychology involves the study of the individual in his or her social environment and includes such topics as personality theory, abnormal psychology, individual differences, developmental psychology and social psychology. Read More
  • Biological Psychology
  • Biological Psychology involves the study of the biological correlates of behavior and includes such topics as physiological psychology, sensory processes, health psychology and animal behavior. Read More
  • Perception, Cognition, and Cognitive Neuroscience
  • Perception, Cognition & Cognitive Neuroscience involves the study of awareness and thought, and includes such topics as perception, learning, memory and consciousness. Read More
  • Quantitative
  • Quantitative psychology emphasizes applied methods for the analysis of data resulting from psychological experiments and correlational studies. Read More
  • Brown Bag Calendars
  • Each Research Area has brown bag presentations. Click below to access the Master Brown Bag Calendar Read More

Psychology 199 Courses

Apply to volunteer as a Research Assistant in a Psychology Department Faculty Member's lab.  Labs are located on campus and at other sites, including the Center for Mind and Brain and the UC Davis Mind Institute and the California National Primate Research Center. Students can receive credit toward the major through Psychology 199

Student Research Participation

Why do we require students to participate in research? Student learning is enhanced when research is experienced first-hand as a subject, especially when the topics focus on human cognition, emotion and behavior. Click here to reach our Sona Student Participation System

  • Search This Site All UCSD Sites Faculty/Staff Search Term
  • Contact & Directions
  • Climate Statement
  • Cognitive Behavioral Neuroscience
  • Cognitive Psychology
  • Developmental Psychology
  • Social Psychology
  • Adjunct Faculty
  • Non-Senate Instructors
  • Researchers
  • Psychology Grads
  • Affiliated Grads
  • New and Prospective Students
  • Honors Program
  • Experiential Learning
  • Programs & Events
  • Psi Chi / Psychology Club
  • Prospective PhD Students
  • Current PhD Students
  • Area Brown Bags
  • Colloquium Series
  • Anderson Distinguished Lecture Series
  • Speaker Videos

Research Areas

Although not bound by any rigid structure, the research interests of the department members can be described by the following categories:

  • Cognitive & Behavioral Neuroscience

Related UCSD Programs

  • Human Development Program
  • Linguistics
  • Cognitive Science
  • Cognitive Science Interdisciplinary Program
  • Computer Science and Engineering
  • SDSU/UCSD Joint Doctoral Program in Clinical Psychology
  • SDSU/UCSD Joint Doctoral Program in Language and Communicative Disorders
  • Ophthalmology
  • Neurosciences
  • UCSD School of Medicine
  • The Scripps Research Institute
  • Salk Institute for Biological Studies
  • Scripps Institution of Oceanography
  • Undergraduate Program
  • Graduate Program

Behaviorism In Psychology

Saul Mcleod, PhD

Editor-in-Chief for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MRes, PhD, University of Manchester

Saul Mcleod, PhD., is a qualified psychology teacher with over 18 years of experience in further and higher education. He has been published in peer-reviewed journals, including the Journal of Clinical Psychology.

Learn about our Editorial Process

Olivia Guy-Evans, MSc

Associate Editor for Simply Psychology

BSc (Hons) Psychology, MSc Psychology of Education

Olivia Guy-Evans is a writer and associate editor for Simply Psychology. She has previously worked in healthcare and educational sectors.

On This Page:

Behaviorism, also known as behavioral learning theory, is a theoretical perspective in psychology that emphasizes the role of learning and observable behaviors in understanding human and animal actions. Behaviorism is a theory of learning that states all behaviors are learned through conditioned interaction with the environment. Thus, behavior is simply a response to environmental stimuli. The behaviorist theory is only concerned with observable stimulus-response behaviors, as they can be studied in a systematic and observable manner. Some of the key figures of the behaviorist approach include B.F. Skinner, known for his work on operant conditioning, and John B. Watson, who established the psychological school of behaviorism.

Key Features
• Stimulus-Response
• Classical Conditioning
• Reinforcement & Punishment
• Objective Measurement
• Reductionism
• Nomothetic
• Law of Effect
• Behaviour should be studied scientifically using experiments.
• Behaviourism is primarily concerned with observable behavior
• The major influence on human behavior is learning from the environment (e.g., conditioning)
• There is little difference between the learning that takes place in humans and that in other animals. Therefore research can be carried out on animals as well as humans.
• Behaviour is the result of stimulus–response (i.e., all behavior, no matter how complex, can be reduced to a simple stimulus–response association).
• Controlled Experiment
• Little Albert
• Thorndike
• Skinner box
• Pavlov’s Dogs
• Bandura Bobo Doll Study
Strengths
• Objective measurement, which can be replicated and peer-reviewed
• Real-life applications (e.g., behavior therapies)
• Clear predictions that can be can be scientifically tested
• Increased our understanding of the causes of phobias and attachment
• Ignores mediational processes
• Reductionist – ignores biology
• Deterministic (little free will)
• Experiments – low ecological validity
• Humanism – can’t compare animals to humans
• Freud – people not born a blank slate

Principles of Behaviorism

The behaviorist movement began in 1913 when John B. Watson wrote an article entitled Psychology as the behaviorist views it , which set out several underlying assumptions regarding methodology and behavioral analysis:

All behavior is learned from the environment:

One assumption of the learning approach is that all behaviors are learned from the environment. They can be learned through classical conditioning, learning by association, or through operant conditioning, learning by consequences.

Behaviorism emphasizes the role of environmental factors in influencing behavior to the near exclusion of innate or inherited factors. This amounts essentially to a focus on learning. Therefore, when born, our mind is “tabula rasa” (a blank slate).

Classical conditioning refers to learning by association, and involves the conditioning of innate bodily reflexes with new stimuli.

Pavlov’s Experiment

Ivan Pavlov showed that dogs could be classically conditioned to salivate at the sound of a bell if that sound was repeatedly presented while they were given food.

Pavlov

He first presented the dogs with the sound of a bell; they did not salivate so this was a neutral stimulus. Then he presented them with food, they salivated.

The food was an unconditioned stimulus and salivation was an unconditioned (innate) response.

Pavlov then repeatedly presented the dogs with the sound of the bell first and then the food (pairing) after a few repetitions, the dogs salivated when they heard the sound of the bell.

The bell had become the conditioned stimulus and salivation had become the conditioned response.

Examples of classical conditioning applied to real life include:

  • taste aversion – using derivations of classical conditioning, it is possible to explain how people develop aversions to particular foods
  • learned emotions – such as love for parents, were explained as paired associations with the stimulation they provide
  • advertising – we readily associate attractive images with the products they are selling
  • phobias – classical conditioning is seen as the mechanism by which – we acquire many of these irrational fears.

Skinner argued that learning is an active process and occurs through operant conditioning . When humans and animals act on and in their environmental consequences, follow these behaviors. 

If the consequences are pleasant, they repeat the behavior, but if the consequences are unpleasant, they do not.

Behavior is the result of stimulus-response:

Reductionism is the belief that human behavior can be explained by breaking it down into smaller component parts.

Reductionists say that the best way to understand why we behave as we do is to look closely at the very simplest parts that make up our systems, and use the simplest explanations to understand how they work.

Psychology should be seen as a science:

Theories need to be supported by empirical data obtained through careful and controlled observation and measurement of behavior. Watson (1913) stated:

“Psychology as a behaviorist views it is a purely objective experimental branch of natural science. Its theoretical goal is … prediction and control.” (p. 158).

The components of a theory should be as simple as possible. Behaviorists propose using operational definitions (defining variables in terms of observable, measurable events).

Behaviorism introduced scientific methods to psychology. Laboratory experiments were used with high control of extraneous variables.

These experiments were replicable, and the data obtained was objective (not influenced by an individual’s judgment or opinion) and measurable. This gave psychology more credibility.

Behaviorism is primarily concerned with observable behavior, as opposed to internal events like thinking and emotion:

The starting point for many behaviorists is a rejection of the introspection (the attempts to “get inside people’s heads”) of the majority of mainstream psychology.

While modern behaviorists often accept the existence of cognitions and emotions, they prefer not to study them as only observable (i.e., external) behavior can be objectively and scientifically measured.

Although theorists of this perspective accept that people have “minds”, they argue that it is never possible to objectively observe people’s thoughts, motives, and meanings – let alone their unconscious yearnings and desires.

Therefore, internal events, such as thinking, should be explained through behavioral terms (or eliminated altogether).

There is little difference between the learning that takes place in humans and that in other animals:

There’s no fundamental (qualitative) distinction between human and animal behavior. Therefore, research can be carried out on animals and humans.

The underlying assumption is that to some degree the laws of behavior are the same for all species and that therefore knowledge gained by studying rats, dogs, cats and other animals can be generalized to humans.

Consequently, rats and pigeons became the primary data source for behaviorists, as their environments could be easily controlled.

Behaviorist Theory

Historically, the most significant distinction between versions of behaviorism is that between Watson’s original methodological behaviorism, and forms of behaviorism later inspired by his work, known collectively as neobehaviorism (e.g., radical behaviorism).

John B Watson: Methodological Behaviorism

Proposed by John B. Watson, methodological behaviorism focuses solely on observable, measurable behaviors and rejects the study of internal mental processes.

Watson argued that thoughts, feelings, and desires cannot be directly observed and, therefore, should not be part of psychological study.

Watson proposed that behaviors can be studied in a systematic and observable manner with no consideration of internal mental states.

He argued that all behaviors in animals or humans are learned, and the environment shapes behavior.

Watson’s article “Psychology as the behaviorist views it” is often referred to as the “behaviorist manifesto,” in which Watson (1913, p. 158) outlines the principles of all behaviorists:

“Psychology as the behaviorist views it is a purely objective experimental branch of natural science. Its theoretical goal is the prediction and control of behavior. Introspection forms no essential part of its methods, nor is the scientific value of its data dependent upon the readiness with which they lend themselves to interpretation in terms of consciousness.”

In his efforts to get a unitary scheme of animal response, the behaviorist recognizes no dividing line between man and brute.

Man’s behavior, with all of its refinement and complexity, forms only a part of the behaviorist’s total scheme of investigation.

This behavioral perspective laid the groundwork for further behavioral studies like B.F’s. Skinner who introduced the concept of operant conditioning.

B.F Skinner: Radical Behaviorism

Radical behaviorism was founded by B.F Skinner , who agreed with the assumption of methodological behaviorism that the goal of psychology should be to predict and control behavior.

Radical Behaviorism expands upon earlier forms of behaviorism by incorporating internal events such as thoughts, emotions, and feelings as part of the behavioral process.

Unlike methodological behaviorism, which asserts that only observable behaviors should be studied, radical behaviorism accepts that these internal events occur and influence behavior.

However, it maintains that they should be considered part of the environmental context and are subject to the same laws of learning and adaptation as overt behaviors.

Another important distinction between methodological and radical behaviorism concerns the extent to which environmental factors influence behavior. Watson’s (1913) methodological behaviorism asserts the mind is a tabula rasa (a blank slate) at birth.

Unlike methodological behaviorism, radical behaviorism recognizes the role of genes and biological components in behavior, accepting that organisms are born with innate behaviors.

Bandura: Social Learning

Behaviorism has undergone many transformations since John Watson developed it in the early part of the twentieth century.

Social learning theory is a more recent extension of behaviorism that emphasizes the role of plans and expectations in people’s behavior.

One more recent extension of this approach has been the development of social learning theory, which emphasizes the role of plans and expectations in people’s behavior.

Under social learning theory , people were no longer seen as passive victims of the environment, but rather they were seen as self-reflecting and thoughtful.

The theory is often called a bridge between behaviorist and cognitive learning theories because it encompasses attention, memory, and motivation.

Applications

Mental health.

Behaviorism theorized that abnormal behavior and mental illness stem from faulty learning processes rather than internal conflicts or unconscious forces, as psychoanalysis claimed.

Based on behaviorism, behavior therapy aims to replace maladaptive behaviors with more constructive ones through techniques like systematic desensitization, aversion therapy, and token economies. Systematic desensitization helps phobia patients gradually confront feared objects.

The behaviorist approach has been used in treating phobias. The individual with the phobia is taught relaxation techniques and then makes a hierarchy of fear from the least frightening to the most frightening features of the phobic object.

He then is presented with the stimuli in that order and learns to associate (classical conditioning) the stimuli with a relaxation response. This is counter-conditioning.

Aversion therapy associates unpleasant stimuli with unwanted habits to discourage them. Token economies reinforce desired actions by providing tokens redeemable for rewards.

The implications of classical conditioning in the classroom are less important than those of  operant conditioning , but there is still a need for teachers to try to make sure that students associate positive emotional experiences with learning.

If a student associates negative emotional experiences with school, then this can obviously have bad results, such as creating a school phobia.

For example, if a student is bullied at school, they may learn to associate the school with fear. It could also explain why some students show a particular dislike of certain subjects that continue throughout their academic career. This could happen if a teacher humiliates or punishes a student in class.

Cue reactivity is the theory that people associate situations (e.g., meeting with friends)/ places (e.g., pub) with the rewarding effects of nicotine, and these cues can trigger a feeling of craving (Carter & Tiffany, 1999).

These factors become smoking-related cues. Prolonged use of nicotine creates an association between these factors and smoking based on classical conditioning.

Nicotine is the unconditioned stimulus (UCS), and the pleasure caused by the sudden increase in dopamine levels is the unconditioned response (UCR). Following this increase, the brain tries to lower the dopamine back to a normal level.

The stimuli that have become associated with nicotine were neutral stimuli (NS) before “learning” took place but they became conditioned stimuli (CS), with repeated pairings. They can produce the conditioned response (CR).

However, if the brain has not received nicotine, the levels of dopamine drop and the individual experiences withdrawal symptoms, therefore, is more likely to feel the need to smoke in the presence of the cues that have become associated with the use of nicotine.

Issues & Debates

Free will vs. determinism.

Behaviorism tends to overemphasize the role of the environment in shaping behavior, suggesting that behavior is primarily determined by external factors such as stimuli, reinforcement, and punishment.

Strong determinism of the behavioral approach as all behavior is learned from our environment through classical and operant conditioning. We are the total sum of our previous conditioning.

The social learning approach, while still emphasizing the role of the environment, recognizes an element of choice in whether we imitate a behavior or not, reflecting a softer form of determinism.

This environmental determinism can lead to underestimating the influence of genetic, biological, and individual factors on behavior.

Behaviorism may provide an incomplete or overly simplistic account of human behavior by neglecting the role of internal processes and individual differences.

Nature vs. Nurture

Behaviorism strongly supports the nature vs. nurture debate , arguing that behavior is primarily learned from the environment.

The social learning theory, which builds upon behaviorist principles, is also on the nurture side, emphasizing the role of observational learning and the influence of role models in shaping behavior.

According to the behaviorist approach, apart from a few innate reflexes and the capacity for learning, all complex behavior is learned from the environment, minimizing the role of genetic or biological factors.

Holism vs. Reductionism

The behaviorist approach and social learning theory are reductionist in nature, as they seek to break down complex behaviors into smaller, more manageable parts for study.

Behaviorists believe that all behavior, regardless of its complexity, can be reduced to the fundamental processes of conditioning, such as classical and operant conditioning.

By focusing on the isolation and manipulation of specific variables, behaviorism aims to identify the basic principles and mechanisms that govern behavior, rather than considering behavior as an irreducible whole.

Idiographic vs. Nomothetic

The behaviorist approach is primarily nomothetic, as it seeks to identify universal laws and principles that govern behavior across all individuals.

It is a nomothetic approach as it views all behavior governed by the same laws of conditioning.

By focusing on these universal principles, behaviorism aims to develop a general theory of behavior that can be applied to all individuals, rather than focusing on the unique experiences and characteristics of each person.

However, it does account for individual differences and explains them in terms of differences in the history of conditioning.

1. Scientific Methodology

Behaviorism emphasizes observable and measurable behaviors, leading to a more scientific and objective approach to studying psychology.

This approach allows for greater objectivity and replicability in psychological research, as behaviors can be quantified and studied systematically.

By emphasizing scientific methods, behaviorism has contributed to the development of psychology as a more rigorous and evidence-based discipline.

2. Empirical Support

Behaviorism has experimental support: Pavlov showed that classical conditioning leads to learning by association.

Watson and Rayner showed that phobias could be learned through classical conditioning in the “Little Albert” experiment.

3. Parsimony

Behaviorist explanations are often simpler and more straightforward than those of other approaches, as they focus on observable behaviors rather than internal mental processes.

According to the law of parsimony, the fewer assumptions a theory makes, the better and the more credible it is. Therefore, behaviorism looks for simple explanations of human behavior from a scientific standpoint.

4. Practical Applications

Behaviorist principles have been successfully applied in various real-world settings, such as clinical therapy, educational interventions, and organizational behavior management.

Techniques like behavior modification, contingency management, and reinforcement schedules have proven effective in modifying problematic behaviors and promoting desired outcomes.

The behaviorist approach has been used in the treatment of phobias, as well as systematic desensitization .

The practical focus of behaviorism has led to the development of evidence-based interventions that can directly benefit individuals and society.

1. Overreliance on Animal Studies

Many of the experiments carried out were done on animals; we are different cognitively and physiologically. Humans have different social norms and moral values that mediate the effects of the environment.

Therefore, people might behave differently from animals, so the laws and principles derived from these experiments might apply more to animals than humans.

2. Difficulty Explaining Spontaneous Behaviors

Behaviorism struggles to explain behaviors that seem to emerge without any apparent environmental triggers or reinforcement history.

Spontaneous behaviors, such as creative insights, sudden changes in preferences, or unprompted acts of kindness, are difficult to account for within a strict behaviorist framework.

The inability to fully explain these behaviors highlights the limitations of focusing solely on observable stimuli and responses, and suggests the need for a more comprehensive approach that considers internal mental processes and individual agency.

3. Neglect of Individual Differences

Behaviorism often assumes that the same learning principles and environmental contingencies apply to all individuals, regardless of their unique experiences, personalities, and cultural backgrounds.

This “one size fits all” approach may overlook important individual differences that shape behavior and influence responses to interventions.

By failing to account for the diversity of human experience and the role of individual factors in shaping behavior, behaviorism may provide an oversimplified or incomplete understanding of psychological phenomena.

Humanism rejects the nomothetic approach of behaviorism. It views humans as unique and believes humans cannot be compared with animals (who aren’t susceptible to demand characteristics).

This is known as an idiographic approach.

4. Limited Explanations for Complex Behaviors

Behaviorist principles may struggle to fully explain more complex human behaviors, such as language acquisition, creativity, problem-solving, and abstract thinking.

These behaviors often involve higher-order cognitive processes and symbolic representation, which are difficult to reduce to simple stimulus-response associations or reinforcement contingencies.

The limited explanatory scope of behaviorism has led to the development of alternative approaches, such as cognitive psychology, that aim to provide more comprehensive accounts of complex mental processes.

5. Reductionist: Oversimplification of Behavior

Behaviorism focuses on externally observable behavior, ignoring essential factors like emotions, expectations, and higher-level motivation.

This oversimplified view of the world has led to the development of ‘pop behaviorism, the view that rewards and punishments can change almost anything. 

Therefore, behaviorism only provides a partial account of human behavior that can be objectively viewed. Essential factors like emotions, expectations, and higher-level motivation are not considered or explained. Accepting a behaviorist explanation could prevent further research from other perspectives that could uncover important factors.

For example, the psychodynamic approach (Freud) criticizes behaviorism as it does not consider the unconscious mind’s influence on behavior and instead focuses on externally observable behavior. Freud also rejects the idea that people are born a blank slate (tabula rasa) and states that people are born with instincts (e.g., eros and Thanatos).

Biological psychology states that all behavior has a physical/organic cause. They emphasize the role of nature over nurture. For example, chromosomes and hormones (testosterone) influence our behavior, too, in addition to the environment.

Behaviorism might be seen as underestimating the importance of inborn tendencies. It is clear from research on biological preparedness that the ease with which something is learned is partly due to its links with an organism’s potential survival.

Cognitive psychology states that mediational processes occur between stimulus and response, such as memory , thinking, problem-solving, etc.

Humanistic psychology also assumes that humans have free will (personal agency) to make their own decisions in life and do not follow the deterministic laws of science . 

Despite these criticisms, behaviorism has made significant contributions to psychology. These include insights into learning, language development, and moral and gender development, which have all been explained in terms of conditioning.

6. Historical Dominance Questioned

Many textbooks depict behaviorism as dominating and defining psychology in the mid-20th century before declining in the late 1950s with the “cognitive revolution.”

However, the empirical basis for claims about behaviorism’s prominence and decline has been limited.

Wide-scope claims about behaviorism are often based on small, unrepresentative samples of historical data. This raises the question – to what extent was behaviorism actually dominant in American psychology?

To address this question, Braat et al. (2020) conducted a quantitative bibliometric analysis of 119,278 articles published in American psychology journals from 1920-1970.

They generated cocitation networks, mapping similarities between frequently cited authors, and co-occurrence networks of frequently used title terms, for each decade.

This allowed them to examine the structure and development of psychology fields without relying on predefined behavioral/non-behavioral categories.

Key findings:

  • In no decade did behaviorist authors belong to the most prominent citation clusters. Even a combined “behaviorist” cluster accounted for max. 28% of highly cited authors.
  • The main focus was measuring personality/mental abilities – those clusters were consistently larger than behaviorist ones.
  • Between 1920 and 1930, Watson was a prominent author, but behaviorism was a small (19%) slice of psychology. Larger clusters were mental testing and Gestalt psychology.
  • From the 1930s, behaviorism split into two clusters, possibly reflecting “classical” vs. “neobehaviorist” approaches. However, the combined behaviorist cluster was still smaller than mental testing and Gestalt clusters.
  • The influence of behaviorism did not dramatically decline after 1950. The behaviorist cluster was stable at 28% during the 1940s-60s, and its citation count quadrupled.
  • Contrary to narratives, Skinner was not highly cited in the 1950s-60s – he did not dominate behaviorism after WWII.
  • Analyses challenge assumptions that behaviorism was the single dominant force in mid-20th-century psychology. The story was more diverse.

However, behaviorist vocabulary became more prominent over time in title term analyses. This suggests behaviorists were influential in shaping psychological research agendas, if not fully dominating the field.

Overall, quantitative analyses provide a richer perspective on the development of behaviorism and 20th-century psychology. Claims that behaviorism “rose and fell” as psychology’s single dominant school appear too simplistic.

Psychology was more multifaceted, with behaviorism as one of several influential but not controlling approaches. The narrative requires reappraisal.

Historical Timeline

  • Pavlov (1897) published the results of an experiment on conditioning after originally studying digestion in dogs.
  • Watson (1913) launches the behavioral school of psychology, publishing an article, Psychology as the behaviorist views it .
  • Watson and Rayner (1920) conditioned an orphan called Albert B (aka Little Albert ) to fear a white rat.
  • Thorndike (1905) formalized the Law of Effect .
  • Skinner (1938) wrote The Behavior of Organisms and introduced the concepts of operant conditioning and shaping.
  • Clark Hull’s (1943) Principles of Behavior was published.
  • B.F. Skinner (1948) published Walden Two , describing a utopian society founded upon behaviorist principles.
  • Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior began in 1958. 
  • Chomsky (1959) published his criticism of Skinner’s behaviorism, Review of Verbal Behavior .
  • Bandura (1963) published a book called the Social Leaning Theory and Personality development which combines both cognitive and behavioral frameworks.
  • B.F. Skinner (1971) published his book, Beyond Freedom and Dignity , where he argues that free will is an illusion.
  • 1980s-1990s – The development of behavior therapy and the increasing integration of cognitive and behavioral approaches in psychology.
  • 2002 – The Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapies (ABCT) was formed through the merger of the Association for Advancement of Behavior Therapy (AABT) and the American Association of Cognitive and Behavioral Therapies (AACBT)

Bandura, A., & Walters, R. H. (1963). Social learning and personality development . New York: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Braat, M., Engelen, J., van Gemert, T., & Verhaegh, S. (2020). The rise and fall of behaviorism: The narrative and the numbers. History of Psychology, 23 (3), 252-280.

Carter, B. L., & Tiffany, S. T. (1999). Meta‐analysis of cue‐reactivity in addiction research.  Addiction ,  94 (3), 327-340.

Chomsky, N. (1959). A review of BF Skinner’s Verbal Behavior . Language, 35(1) , 26-58.

Holland, J. G. (1978). BEHAVIORISM: PART OF THE PROBLEM OR PART OF THE SOLUTION?  Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis ,  11 (1), 163-174.

Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior: An introduction to behavior theory . New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Pavlov, I. P. (1897). The work of the digestive glands . London: Griffin.

Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis . New York: Appleton-Century.

Skinner, B. F. (1948). Walden two. New York: Macmillan.

Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond freedom and dignity . New York: Knopf.

Thorndike, E. L. (1905). The elements of psychology . New York: A. G. Seiler.

Watson, J. B. (1913). Psychology as the behaviorist views it . Psychological Review, 20 , 158-178.

Watson, J. B. (1930). Behaviorism (revised edition). University of Chicago Press.

Watson, J. B., & Rayner, R. (1920). Conditioned emotional reactions . Journal of Experimental Psychology, 3 , 1, pp. 1–14.

What is the theory of behaviorism?

What is behaviorism with an example.

An example of behaviorism is using systematic desensitization in the treatment of phobias. The individual with the phobia is taught relaxation techniques and then makes a hierarchy of fear from the least frightening to the most frightening features of the phobic object.

How behaviorism is used in the classroom?

In the conventional learning situation, behaviorist pedagogy applies largely to issues of class and student management, rather than to learning content.

It is very relevant to shaping skill performance. For example, unwanted behaviors, such as tardiness and dominating class discussions, can be extinguished by being ignored by the teacher (rather than being reinforced by having attention drawn to them).

Who founded behaviorism?

John B. Watson founded behaviorism. Watson proposed that psychology should abandon its focus on mental processes, which he believed were impossible to observe and measure objectively, and focus solely on observable behaviors.

His ideas, published in a famous article “ Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It ” in 1913, marked the formal start of behaviorism as a major school of psychological thought.

Is behavior analysis the same as behaviorism?

No, behavior analysis and behaviorism are not the same. Behaviorism is a broader philosophical approach to psychology emphasizing observable behaviors over internal events like thoughts and emotions.

Behavior analysis , specifically applied behavior analysis (ABA), is a scientific discipline and set of methods derived from behaviorist principles, used to understand and change specific behaviors, often employed in therapeutic contexts, such as with autism treatment.

Print Friendly, PDF & Email

Related Articles

Soft Determinism In Psychology

Soft Determinism In Psychology

Branches of Psychology

Branches of Psychology

Social Action Theory (Weber): Definition & Examples

Social Action Theory (Weber): Definition & Examples

Attachment Styles and How They Affect Adult Relationships

Adult Attachment , Personality , Psychology , Relationships

Attachment Styles and How They Affect Adult Relationships

Big Five Personality Traits: The 5-Factor Model of Personality

Personality , Psychology

Big Five Personality Traits: The 5-Factor Model of Personality

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory

Learning Theories , Psychology , Social Science

Albert Bandura’s Social Learning Theory

Relationship between emotional awareness and self-acceptance: the mediating role of emotion regulation strategies

  • Published: 07 June 2024

Cite this article

research areas in cognitive psychology

  • Ziming Huang 1 ,
  • Shulin Chen   ORCID: orcid.org/0000-0003-4114-3297 1 &
  • Hang Chen 2  

Self-acceptance is known as a strong indicator of positive mental health, and previous studies have identified the relationship between self-acceptance and emotion. However, there are still some research gaps in the relationship between self-acceptance and key emotional variables such as emotional awareness and emotional regulation strategies. The exploration of this part is also more conducive to the future design of effective ways to improve self-acceptance. Considering the Gross emotion regulation model, the present study aimed to examine the relationship between emotional awareness and self-acceptance, as well as the mediating effect of emotion regulation strategies. Data were collected from 419 college students in Zhejiang Province, China. Results showed that emotional awareness (IV) was positively associated with self-acceptance (DV), and both cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression partially mediated the influence of emotional awareness on self-acceptance. The results suggest an underlying mechanism between emotional awareness and self-acceptance with emotion regulation strategies as robust factors. Theoretically, all of the results had significant ramifications and supplemented the empirical findings in the area of emotional health and self-acceptance. The mediation model also offers emotion-based innovation for approaches to improve self-acceptance, which has practical implications.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price includes VAT (Russian Federation)

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Rent this article via DeepDyve

Institutional subscriptions

research areas in cognitive psychology

Data availability

The data of this paper is collected by the author on campus, and has a complete database. As the database is related to mental health variables and concerns personal privacy, it will not be made public. Relevant information can be obtained from the author of this article on request.

Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in personality . Holt, Rinehart and Winston.

Bagby, M., Taylor, G. J., & Ryan, D. (1986). Toronto alexithymia scale: relationship with personality and psychopathology measures. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics , 45 (4), 207–215.

Article   PubMed   Google Scholar  

Berger, E. M. (1952). The relation between expressed acceptance of self and expressed acceptance of others 1 .

Chamberlain, J. M., & Haaga, D. A. (2001). Unconditional self-acceptance and psychological health. Journal of Rational-Emotive and Cognitive-Behavior Therapy , 19 (3), 163–176.

Article   Google Scholar  

Cong, Z., & Gao, W. (1999). Compilation of self-acceptance questionnaire and its reliability and validity test. Chinese Journal of Behavioral Medicine and Brain Sciences , 01 , 20–22.

Google Scholar  

Cucu-Ciuhan, G., & Dumitru, I. E. (2017). Unconditional self-acceptance, functional and dysfunctional negative emotions, and self-esteem as predictors for depression in adolescents: A brief pilot study conducted in Romania. Journal of Experiential Psychotherapy/Revista de PSIHOterapie Experientiala , 20 (4).

Di Maggio, R., Zappulla, C., & Pace, U. (2016). The relationship between emotion knowledge, emotion regulation and adjustment in preschoolers: A mediation model. Journal of Child and Family Studies , 25 , 2626–2635.

Elliott, R., Watson, J. C., Goldman, R. N., & Greenberg, L. S. (2004). Learning emotion-focused therapy: The process-experiential approach to change . American Psychological Association.

Gross, J. J. (1998). The emerging field of emotion regulation: An integrative review. Review of General Psychology , 2 (3), 271–299.

Gross, J. J. (2001). Emotion regulation in adulthood: Timing is everything. Current Directions in Psychological Science , 10 (6), 214–219.

Gross, J. J. (2002). Emotion regulation: Affective, cognitive, and social consequences. Psychophysiology , 39 (3), 281–291.

Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K. D., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. Encyclopedia of Psychotherapy , 9 (1), 1–8.

Hill, C. E. (2009). Helping skills: Facilitating, exploration, insight, and action . American Psychological Association.

Lane, R. D. (2000). Levels of emotional awareness: Neurological, psychological, and social perspectives .

Lane, R. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. The American Journal of Psychiatry .

Lane, R. D., Quinlan, D. M., Schwartz, G. E., Walker, P. A., & Zeitlin, S. B. (1990). The levels of emotional awareness scale: A cognitive-developmental measure of emotion. Journal of Personality Assessment , 55 (1–2), 124–134.

Lei, X. Y., Xiao, L. M., Liu, Y. N., & Li, Y. M. (2016). Prevalence of depression among Chinese University students: A meta-analysis. PloS One , 11 (4), e0153454.

Li, H. (2021). College students’ perfectionism on the influence of negative emotions (a master’s degree thesis, sichuan normal university).

Li, J. (2022). The status and influencing factors of self-acceptance in vocational college students. Monthly Journal of Psychology , 17 , 17–19. https://doi.org/10.19738/j.cnki.psy.2022.17.005

Lundh, L. G., Johnsson, A., Sundqvist, K., & Olsson, H. (2002). Alexithymia, memory of emotion, emotional awareness, and perfectionism. Emotion , 2 (4), 361.

MacInnes, D. L. (2006). Self-esteem and self‐acceptance: An examination into their relationship and their effect on psychological health. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing , 13 (5), 483–489.

McRae, K., Misra, S., Prasad, A. K., Pereira, S. C., & Gross, J. J. (2012). Bottom-up and top-down emotion generation: Implications for emotion regulation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience , 7 (3), 253–262.

Pasaribu, P. E., & Zarfiel, M. D. (2019, August). Cognitive behavioral therapy treatment for reducing stress: A case study of self-acceptance in an early adult college student. In 2nd International Conference on Intervention and Applied Psychology (ICIAP 2018) (pp. 631–644). Atlantis Press.

Schoenleber, M., & Gratz, K. L. (2018). Self-acceptance group therapy: A transdiagnostic, cognitive-behavioral treatment for shame. Cognitive and Behavioral Practice , 25 (1), 75–86.

Stanković, S., Matić, M., Vukosavljević-Gvozden, T., & Opačić, G. (2015). Frustration intolerance and unconditional self-acceptance as mediators of the relationship between perfectionism and depression. Psihologija , 48 (2), 101–117.

Wang, H. (2013). Characteristics and optimization of preservice teachers’ emotional awareness . (Doctoral dissertation, Shanghai Normal University).

Wang, X. (2018). Effects of emotional awareness group counseling on emotional awareness and mental health of junior high school students. (Doctoral dissertation, Hunan Normal University).

Wang, Z. (2021). Social adjustment and anxiety of middle school students under the epidemic situation (Master’s Thesis, Central China Normal University).

Wang, L., Liu, H., & Li, Z., Du Wei (2007). Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the emotional regulation questionnaire. Chinese Journal of Health Psychology , 15 (6), 3.

Wang, H., Cheng, J., & Huang Fei. (2016). Emotional awareness of college students and its relationship with emotion regulation. Journal of Huangshan University , 18 (2), 3.

Wang, J., Dai, W., & Jia, N. (2020). The influence of paternal parenting style on test anxiety and social anxiety in senior high school students: The mediating role of self-acceptance. Journal of School Psychology , 18 (6), 5.

Wu, X., Wang, J., Zhou, M., Tang, R., Chen, C., Wang, Y., & Zhang, X. (2021). Current status and influencing factors of self-acceptance among medical college students. Chinese Higher Medical Education , 10 , 45–46.

Xu, J., Zhou, Q., Li, J., Wang, H., & Yao, B. (2018). The relationship between emotional intelligence and learning burnout in vocational nursing students: The mediating role of self-acceptance. Chinese Journal of Health Management , 35 (7), 4.

Yao, K. (2014). A study on the intervention of restrictive group sand table on college students’ self-acceptance. (Doctoral dissertation, Yangzhou University).

Zhang, X., Baikti, A., & Zhang, X. (2012). Emotional regulation and self-acceptance in patients with cervical cancer. Nursing Practice and Research , 9 (22), 2.

Zhang, M., Zhang, J., Zhang, F., Zhang, L., & Feng, D. (2018). Prevalence of psychological distress and the effects of resilience and perceived social support among Chinese college students: Does gender make a difference? Psychiatry Research , 267 , 409–413.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank all the participants in the study. We thank LetPub ( www.letpub.com ) for its linguistic assistance during the preparation of this manuscript.

Author information

Authors and affiliations.

Department of Psychological and Behavioral Science, Zhejiang University, Zhejiang University, No 148 Tianmushan Rd, Hangzhou, 310028, China

Ziming Huang & Shulin Chen

College of Biomedical Engineering and Instrument Science, Zhejiang University, No 38 Zheda Rd, Hangzhou, 310028, China

You can also search for this author in PubMed   Google Scholar

Corresponding authors

Correspondence to Shulin Chen or Hang Chen .

Ethics declarations

Ethical approval and consent to participate.

The study was approved by the Institutional Review Board (IRB) of the Department of psychology and behavioral science, Zhejiang University (IRB approval code: [2021]069). Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of interest

The authors have no relevant financial or non-financial interests to disclose.

Additional information

Publisher’s note.

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Springer Nature or its licensor (e.g. a society or other partner) holds exclusive rights to this article under a publishing agreement with the author(s) or other rightsholder(s); author self-archiving of the accepted manuscript version of this article is solely governed by the terms of such publishing agreement and applicable law.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Huang, Z., Chen, S. & Chen, H. Relationship between emotional awareness and self-acceptance: the mediating role of emotion regulation strategies. Curr Psychol (2024). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-05945-2

Download citation

Accepted : 27 March 2024

Published : 07 June 2024

DOI : https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-024-05945-2

Share this article

Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:

Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative

  • Self-acceptance
  • Emotional awareness
  • Emotion regulation strategies
  • Emotional health
  • Cognitive reappraisal
  • Expressive suppression
  • Find a journal
  • Publish with us
  • Track your research

Aleshyn Andrei Shutterstock

Are You Emotionally Numb?

A Key to Healing

  • Find a Therapist

Verified by Psychology Today

  • Psychiatrists
  • Treatment Centers
  • Support Groups

Professionals

Therapists Sign Up | Login

Psychology Today: Health, Help, Happiness + Find a Therapist

Today’s essentials today’s essential reads.

research areas in cognitive psychology

A New Peek Behind the Mirror of Narcissism

Susan Krauss Whitbourne PhD, ABPP on June 8, 2024 in Fulfillment at Any Age

Narcissists are seen as antagonistic and attention-grabbing, but is that always true? A new study shows 5 ways to consider narcissism's true qualities.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Consumer Behavior

Reviews with pictures are (sometimes) helpful.

Art Markman Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Ulterior Motives

Many factors may make a review more or less helpful. How do photos help a review have more impact? New research explores this question.

research areas in cognitive psychology

How Old Is Too Old to Look Young?

Wendy L. Patrick, J.D., Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Why Bad Looks Good

What do younger people think about your attempts to look more like them? Research has some interesting answers.

research areas in cognitive psychology

The New Fentanyl and Speedballing Crisis of 2024

Mark S. Gold M.D. on June 6, 2024 in Addiction Outlook

Smoking fentanyl and speedballing is a new dangerous trend. Newer overdose reversal protocols and recovery medications are needed while we try to improve access and initiation.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Artificial Intelligence

Can llms become our new moral compass.

John Nosta on June 8, 2024 in The Digital Self

New data suggest LLMs can provide moral advice rated more thoughtful, trustworthy, and correct than laypeople and expert ethicists.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Adult Children of Polyamorous Parents

Elisabeth A. Sheff Ph.D., CSE on June 7, 2024 in The Polyamorists Next Door

This post explores the three most common reactions that adult children have to their parents who come out to them as polyamorous.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Emotion Regulation

The key to effective discipline.

Kat Scherer, Ph.D., and Elizabeth Sylvester, Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Relationship-Based Parenting

Children's misbehavior is stressful, and adult emotional reactivity can worsen it. Sometimes, our attempts to discipline children can backfire, escalating upset and acting out.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Every Parent's Biggest Regret

Jeffrey Bernstein Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Liking the Child You Love

Discover the lessons learned from parents who wish they could turn back time to fix their biggest parenting regret.

Just No to Politics

Why Politics Get Nasty

Heather Lench Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in The Emotional Toolkit

Ever wonder why arguments about politics get so heated? The political messages we all watch could hold the answer.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Balancing Needs as a Caregiver

Kristi Rendahl DPA on June 7, 2024 in The Family Caregiver

Caregiving is fundamentally about caring for someone, but it shouldn't be done at the expense of caregivers themselves. Caregiver wellness is also a top priority.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Growth Mindset

How a growth mindset can enhance romantic relationships.

Jessica Koehler Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Beyond School Walls

How can adopting a growth mindset transform your romantic relationship into a thriving, resilient partnership?

research areas in cognitive psychology

How Religious Themes Can Aid Those With Mental Illness

David T. Culkin Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in OCD and Marriage

Religious experience can remind us that we are not alone, a key to healing in a divided world.

research areas in cognitive psychology

The Shadow Self

Iskra Fileva Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in The Philosopher's Diaries

Sometimes, details about our lives that we keep private become a source of anxiety and shame. Silence weighs on us. Our shadow self haunts us.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Stress and Decision Making When Dealing With Infertility

Lisa Schuman on June 7, 2024 in Building Your Family

Everyone knows that going through infertility is stressful but perhaps we don’t consider that it may be so stressful that it can lead you to make unhelpful decisions.

research areas in cognitive psychology

What We Tell Ourselves Hard Experiences "Mean" Matters a Lot

John-Manuel Andriote on June 7, 2024 in Stonewall Strong

Personal Perspective: A trip to Paris could have been ruined on the first day if I had chosen to let it. Life happens. All we get to choose is whether to accept that fact or not.

research areas in cognitive psychology

The Power of Never Giving Up

Robert Puff Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Meditation for Modern Life

How to overcome your past, embrace hope, and find lasting happiness.

research areas in cognitive psychology

  • Relationships

How the Big 5 Personality Traits Shape Romantic Success

Frank J. Ninivaggi M.D., DLF-A.P.A. on June 7, 2024 in Envy This!

Why the Big Five traits matter for couples.

research areas in cognitive psychology

How Much Should You Tell Your Kids About Adult Problems?

Robert Taibbi L.C.S.W. on June 7, 2024 in Fixing Families

Children and teens are naturally sensitive to their parent's world. The challenge is knowing how much or how little to say as a parent. Here are some guidelines.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Psychopharmacology

The normalization of anxiety medication: what is the impact.

Michele Leno Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Mind Matters

Psychological approaches to anxiety treatment have become more personalized and holistic as stigmas surrounding mental health have waned in recent years.

Mimage Shutterstock

Find Your Life’s Fulfillment

In Love and Work

research areas in cognitive psychology

Interpersonal Emotional Regulation in a Digital World

Phil Reed D.Phil. on June 7, 2024 in Digital World, Real World

Why digital communication may hurt emotional regulation strategies.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Climate Anxiety

Child mental health and climate change adaptation.

Ilan Kelman Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Disaster By Choice

A review of 160 national climate change adaptation policies found a complete absence of addressing child mental health.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Lighten Your (Cognitive) Load

Carlos Alós-Ferrer Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Decisions and the Brain

If you are always in a hurry and have too much on your mind, you might be setting yourself up for failure. Here is why and how to stop.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Harnessing the Power of Behavior to Feel Better

James Gross Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Feel Better

Learn how to harness your behavior to help you feel better, overcome burnout, depression, and anxiety, and get closer to living the life you want.

research areas in cognitive psychology

8 Questions to Ask Before Giving Up on Your Marriage

Melissa Orlov on June 7, 2024 in May I Have Your Attention

If you've been struggling in your relationship, you might wonder if you will ever be able to be happy again. But calling it quits is hard. Explore these questions first.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Assessing Suicidality: Beyond Issues of Don’t Ask Don’t Tell

Kyle D. Killian Ph.D., LMFT on June 7, 2024 in Intersections

Accurate clinical information is needed for good decision-making about suicide.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Rekindling Love: How Deep Listening Can Rescue Relationship

Guy Itzchakov Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in The Listening Lens

Discover how deep listening saved a marriage and reignited connection. Inspired by Thich Nhat Hanh, it's a tale of empathy and lasting love.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Why Does Making Friends Get Harder as We Age?

Kaytee Gillis, LCSW-BACS on June 7, 2024 in Invisible Bruises

While building friendships in childhood may have seemed effortless, many individuals find that making new friends becomes noticeably more difficult as they grow older.

research areas in cognitive psychology

The Process of Putting Yourself First

Justin Kompf Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in A Practical Approach to Addressing Barriers to Physical Activity

Overcome the struggle of putting yourself first. Discover how ingrained emotions and identities might be holding you back from behavior change.

research areas in cognitive psychology

How to Promote Healthy Sleep Habits for Your Teen

Sue Scheff on June 7, 2024 in Shame Nation

Sleep is critical during the teenage years for emotional and physical health. Here are three ways to promote to improve young people's sleep and mental wellness.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Do You Fight, Take Flight, or Freeze?

Diane N Solomon Ph.D., PMHNP-BC, CNM (Ret.) on June 7, 2024 in The Narrative Nurse Practitioner

The fight-flight-or-freeze theory was developed in the early 1900s. Here's an update you need to know.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Neurodiversity

Creating a summertime routine for a neurodivergent child.

Lisa Sheinhouse M.A. on June 7, 2024 in Retrain Your Brain

With a bit of preparation, parents can successfully create and maintain a neurodivergent child's summertime routine.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Building an Empowered Self Amid Identity Fluctuations

Steven Stosny, Ph.D. on June 7, 2024 in Anger in the Age of Entitlement

The most powerful identities are those that most accurately express the whole self.

research areas in cognitive psychology

Personal Perspectives

How my motorcycle crash improved my life.

Judith M. Ford MSW, MFA on June 7, 2024 in Close Calls and Narrow Escapes

My motorcycle crash affected more than just me when help came from unexpected places.

  • Find a Treatment Center
  • Find a Psychiatrist
  • Find a Support Group
  • Find Online Therapy
  • United States
  • Brooklyn, NY
  • Chicago, IL
  • Houston, TX
  • Los Angeles, CA
  • New York, NY
  • Portland, OR
  • San Diego, CA
  • San Francisco, CA
  • Seattle, WA
  • Washington, DC
  • Asperger's
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Chronic Pain
  • Eating Disorders
  • Passive Aggression
  • Personality
  • Goal Setting
  • Positive Psychology
  • Stopping Smoking
  • Low Sexual Desire
  • Child Development
  • Self Tests NEW
  • Therapy Center
  • Diagnosis Dictionary
  • Types of Therapy

May 2024 magazine cover

At any moment, someone’s aggravating behavior or our own bad luck can set us off on an emotional spiral that threatens to derail our entire day. Here’s how we can face our triggers with less reactivity so that we can get on with our lives.

  • Emotional Intelligence
  • Gaslighting
  • Affective Forecasting
  • Neuroscience
  • Bipolar Disorder
  • Therapy Center
  • When To See a Therapist
  • Types of Therapy
  • Best Online Therapy
  • Best Couples Therapy
  • Best Family Therapy
  • Managing Stress
  • Sleep and Dreaming
  • Understanding Emotions
  • Self-Improvement
  • Healthy Relationships
  • Student Resources
  • Personality Types
  • Guided Meditations
  • Verywell Mind Insights
  • 2024 Verywell Mind 25
  • Mental Health in the Classroom
  • Editorial Process
  • Meet Our Review Board
  • Crisis Support

Kendra Cherry, MSEd

  • Author of the " Everything Psychology Book (2nd Edition) "
  • Educational consultant and speaker focused on helping students learn about and enjoy psychology
  • As a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, helped children suffering from emotional disturbances to overcome maladaptive behaviors

If you have ever wondered why people do the things they do, how they become who they are, or what factors influence how people develop, psychology can offer insight and answers. Whether you are a psychology major or a student taking an introductory course in the subject, my goal is to provide useful information and resources to further your understanding and appreciation of psychology.

Kendra Cherry, MS, is an author and educator with more than 15 of years experience helping students make sense of psychology. She is the author of the " Everything Psychology Book (2nd Edition) " and she has published thousands of articles on diverse topics in psychology including mental health, personality, social behavior, child therapy, intelligence, research methods, and much more.

As a psychosocial rehabilitation specialist, Kendra utilized behavioral, cognitive, and socialization strategies to help her young clients cope with family relationships, peer interactions, aggression, social skills, and academic difficulties. Her work has been referenced by numerous media outlets and publications including The New York Times, CNN, Psychology Today, The Telegraph, The Huffington Post, Business Insider, and The Guardian.

She is also the founder of Explore Psychology , an educational website that offers study guides and resources for psychology students.

Kendra Cherry has a Master of Science in education from Boise State University with a primary research interest in educational psychology. She also holds a Bachelor of Science in psychology from Idaho State University with additional coursework in substance use and case management.

About Verywell Mind

Verywell Mind, a Dotdash Meredith Brand , is an award-winning, trusted, and compassionate online resource that provides the guidance you need to improve your mental health and find balance. We take a human approach to health and wellness, and reach more than 150 million readers annually. Verywell Mind content is fact checked and reviewed by our Review Board for accuracy and integrity. Learn more about us and our editorial process .

  • When Is Extreme Happiness a Sign of Something Deeper?
  • Why Are You So Anxious During Test Taking?
  • Inositol: Benefits, Side Effects, Dosage, and Interactions
  • How Depressants Affect Your Body
  • Can Marijuana Cause Psychosis?
  • CBD vs. THC: What's the Difference?
  • What Are Opiates?
  • Mental Health Resources to Support the LGBTQIA+ Community
  • INTP: Introverted, Intuitive, Thinking, Perceiving
  • The Behavioral Psychology Theory That Explains Learned Behavior
  • What Does a Social Worker Do?
  • Does Conventional Wisdom Hinder Creative Thinking?
  • What to Know About the Illusion of Control
  • Gray Divorce: Why Older Couples Are Splitting Up More Often
  • 10 of the Most Famous Psychologists
  • Personality Theories in Psychology
  • Social Comparison Theory in Psychology
  • What Is a Dependent Variable?
  • What It's Like to Have Aphantasia
  • INFJ: Introverted, Intuitive, Feeling, Judging
  • What It Means to Be in a Platonic Relationship
  • What Makes a Person Heroic?
  • What Does Happiness Really Mean?
  • Prenatal Development Stages
  • 'I Don't Need Friends': Why You Might Feel This Way
  • The James-Lange Theory of Emotion
  • How to Stop People-Pleasing
  • What Is Confirmation Bias?
  • An Overview of the American Psychological Association (APA)
  • How Neuroplasticity Works
  • What to Know About Pursuing a Career in Health Psychology
  • B.F. Skinner's Life, Theories, and Influence on Psychology
  • Have I Fallen Out of Love or Am I Depressed?
  • Why Do We Dream?
  • What Does the Color Purple Mean?
  • Operant vs. Classical Conditioning
  • How to Learn More Effectively
  • Major Branches of Psychology
  • How a Life Coach Can Help You
  • Repression in Psychology
  • Sublimation in Psychology
  • What to Know About Mindfulness Meditation
  • Why Toxic Positivity Can Be Harmful
  • Implicit Memory vs. Explicit Memory
  • 60 "How Well Do You Know Me" Questions to Improve Your Relationships
  • What Is a Schema in Psychology?
  • What Is Body Positivity?
  • Trypophobia and the Fear of Holes
  • Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) Overview
  • 6 Types of Relationships and Their Effect on Your Life

IMAGES

  1. What is Cognitive Psychology?

    research areas in cognitive psychology

  2. PPT

    research areas in cognitive psychology

  3. Cognitive Approach In Psychology

    research areas in cognitive psychology

  4. Research Areas within Cognitive Psychology

    research areas in cognitive psychology

  5. Research Methods in Cognitive Psychology

    research areas in cognitive psychology

  6. PPT

    research areas in cognitive psychology

VIDEO

  1. RESEARCH METHODOLOGY IN COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY BY IIT KANPUR

  2. MPC 001 Cognitive Psychology

  3. Research methods in cognitive psychology

  4. UWaterloo Cognitive Neuroscience

  5. COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY LECTURE: INCIDENTAL LEARNING

  6. Definitions of research terms used in psychology

COMMENTS

  1. Cognitive Psychology: Research Areas: Research: Psychological and Brain

    Research in Cognitive Psychology Cognitive psychologists develop a systematic understanding of cognitive processes. These include perception, human learning, attention, categorization, problem solving, decision-making, information processing and retrieval, short and long-term memory and forgetting, sensory encoding, motor control ...

  2. Cognitive

    Cognitive. Faculty in the Cognitive area investigate a wide range of topics including language, meaning and mental representation, perception, learning, memory, decision making and reasoning. Our methods include behavioral experimentation, computational and mathematical modeling, naturalistic observation and cross-cultural fieldwork, and neuro ...

  3. Cognitive Psychology: The Science of How We Think

    MaskotOwner/Getty Images. Cognitive psychology involves the study of internal mental processes—all of the workings inside your brain, including perception, thinking, memory, attention, language, problem-solving, and learning. Cognitive psychology--the study of how people think and process information--helps researchers understand the human brain.

  4. Cognitive Psychology Explores Our Mental Processes

    Topics in Psychology. Explore how scientific research by psychologists can inform our professional lives, family and community relationships, emotional wellness, and more. ... Brain science and cognitive psychology is one of the most versatile psychological specialty areas today — and one of the most in demand. All professions have a ...

  5. Research Areas

    Cognitive Psychology. Cognitive Psychology is the scientific study of mental processes underlying behavior, a broad research area encompassing the study of attention, perception, memory, language, reasoning and problem-solving. These research areas are strongly represented by the faculty of the Cognitive Psychology Program along with research ...

  6. Cognitive Psychology

    In addition to the core faculty in Cognitive Psychology, many faculty members with other primary specializations are also interested in cognitive research. Students are encouraged to read widely both in the cognitive area and in related disciplines, such as computer science, linguistics, philosophy, and statistics.

  7. Home

    Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications publishes new empirical and theoretical work covering all areas of Cognition, with a special emphasis on use-inspired basic research: fundamental research that grows from hypotheses about real-world problems. We expect that authors will be able to explain in a Significance section how their basic research serves to advance our understanding of ...

  8. Cognitive Psychology • UCLA Department of Psychology

    Information about the Cognitive Psychology Graduate Major. This Area's goal is to produce cognitive psychologists who combine general scholarship with sophistication in research design, theory formulation, experimental and computational methods, and scientific communication. ... There are applied research opportunities as well in areas such ...

  9. Cognitive neuroscience

    Cognitive neuroscience is the field of study focusing on the neural substrates of mental processes. It is at the intersection of psychology and neuroscience, but also overlaps with physiological ...

  10. The brain basis of cognition

    The heart of Mesulam's review is that a deep understanding of the genesis and organization of cognitive functions can be achieved by examining neuroanatomical organization. Mesulam discusses a ...

  11. Cognitive Neuroscience

    Cognitive neuroscientists examine the brain processes that come together to inform our thoughts and understanding of the world. Our researchers use a range of approaches, including functional brain imaging and cognitive psychology, to ask fundamental questions about mental processes including how we focus attention, how we process language and music, and how the brain develops […]

  12. Research Areas

    Cognition. Faculty members in Cognition investigate a wide range of areas in Cognitive Science using a variety of methods. Topics include language, scene processing, event segmentation, music, statistical learning, memory and consciousness. Read more about Cognition.

  13. Current Cognitive Neuroscience Research and Technology: Selected Areas

    Cognitive neuroscience and related technologies constitute a multifaceted discipline that is burgeoning on many fronts. Based on the expertise of its members, and realizing that it could not possibly cover the entire range of science within the discipline, the committee chose to discuss three specific areas of interest: (1) challenges to the detection of psychological states and intentions via ...

  14. Cognitive Science

    Associate Professor of Psychology. My research focuses on the cognitive and neural basis of translating abstract thought into words and sentences. I am specifically interested in questions of automaticity and control over the process of language production, and in how people detect and correct errors in their spoken, written, or typed production.

  15. Topics, Methods, and Research-Based Strategies for Teaching ...

    Abstract. In this chapter, we review the basic contents and structure of our courses in cognition and cognitive psychology as well as pedagogical approaches to teaching. Topics range from an historical overview of the areas of science that lead up to the formation of cognitive science to detailed discussions of published articles within each of ...

  16. Cognitive Psychology

    The faculty in cognitive psychology have research programs focused on human memory, language processing, categorization, reasoning, computational modeling, and cognitive aging. The cognitive psychology program emphasizes acquiring exceptionally strong research skills in a domain chosen by the student, working closely with individual faculty members, and using multiple dependent measures to ...

  17. Major Branches of Psychology: 18 Areas of Study

    Cognitive psychology is a psychological area that focuses on internal mental states. This area has continued to grow since it emerged in the 1960s and is centered on the science of how people think, learn, and remember. ... Research in I/O psychology is known as applied research because it seeks to solve real-world problems. These types of ...

  18. Cognitive psychology

    cognitive psychology, Branch of psychology devoted to the study of human cognition, particularly as it affects learning and behaviour. The field grew out of advances in Gestalt, developmental, and comparative psychology and in computer science, particularly information-processing research. Cognitive psychology shares many research interests with cognitive science, and some experts classify it ...

  19. Cognitive Approach In Psychology

    Another strength is that the research conducted in this area of psychology very often has applications in the real world. ... - Neisser's book "Cognitive Psychology" formally established cognitive psychology as a separate area of study, focusing on mental processes such as perception, memory, and thinking.

  20. What Is the Cognitive Psychology Approach? 12 Key Theories

    Cognitive psychology has turned its attention to all aspects of human cognition. And while it is beyond the scope of this article to cover the full breadth or depth of the areas of research, we list several of the most important and fascinating specialties and theories below.

  21. Areas of Research

    The broad goal of cognitive science is to characterize the nature of human knowledge - its forms and content - and how that knowledge is used, processed, and acquired. Active areas of cognitive research in the Department include language, memory, visual perception and cognition, thinking and reasoning, social cognition, decision making, and ...

  22. Psychology Research Areas

    Psychology Research Areas. The UC Davis Department of Psychology contains five major "areas": Developmental, Perception-Cognition, Biological Psychology, Quantitative and Social-Personality. Boundaries between the areas are fluid, and students are encouraged to take seminars in all five.

  23. Research Areas

    Research Areas. Although not bound by any rigid structure, the research interests of the department members can be described by the following categories: Cognitive & Behavioral Neuroscience. Cognitive Psychology. Developmental Psychology. Social Psychology. Expand All.

  24. <em>British Journal of Psychology</em>

    The British Journal of Psychology is the flagship journal of the BPS, publishing cutting-edge, multidisciplinary research across all areas of psychology. Abstract Throughout the day, humans show natural fluctuations in arousal that impact cognitive function. ... Neuropsychology and Cognitive Neurosciences Research Center, Faculty of Health ...

  25. Behaviorism In Psychology

    Behaviorism emphasizes observable and measurable behaviors, leading to a more scientific and objective approach to studying psychology. This approach allows for greater objectivity and replicability in psychological research, as behaviors can be quantified and studied systematically. By emphasizing scientific methods, behaviorism has ...

  26. Relationship between emotional awareness and self-acceptance ...

    Self-acceptance is known as a strong indicator of positive mental health, and previous studies have identified the relationship between self-acceptance and emotion. However, there are still some research gaps in the relationship between self-acceptance and key emotional variables such as emotional awareness and emotional regulation strategies. The exploration of this part is also more ...

  27. Psychology Today: Health, Help, Happiness + Find a Therapist

    View the latest from the world of psychology: from behavioral research to practical guidance on relationships, mental health and addiction. Find help from our directory of therapists ...

  28. Kendra Cherry, MSEd

    Experience. Kendra Cherry, MS, is an author and educator with more than 15 of years experience helping students make sense of psychology. She is the author of the "Everything Psychology Book (2nd Edition)" and she has published thousands of articles on diverse topics in psychology including mental health, personality, social behavior, child therapy, intelligence, research methods, and much more.

  29. Browse journals and books

    The Nuclear Research Foundation School Certificate Integrated, Volume 2. Book • 1966. Abschlusskurs Sonografie der Bewegungsorgane First Edition. Book • 2024. ... Access All Areas. A real world guide to gigging and touring. Book • 2007. Access Control and Personal Identification Systems. Book • 1988. Accident Analysis & Prevention.